No AccessLanguage, Speech, and Hearing Services in SchoolsClinical Forum1 Jan 2010

Genetic Influences on Language, Reading, and Mathematics Skills in a National Sample: An Analysis Using the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth

    Purpose

    The present study had two purposes: provide an illustration of use of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 Children’s (CNLSY; U.S. Department of Labor, 2009) database and use the database to seek convergent evidence regarding the magnitude and significance of genetic effects influencing low and typical performers on measures of language, reading, and mathematics.

    Methods

    A kinship algorithm that assigned a degree of genetic relatedness to all available pairings was applied to the 1994 wave of the CNLSY sample. Four cognitive achievement outcomes related to language, reading, and mathematics were analyzed across the general sample as well as for children selected below the lowest 20th percentile.

    Results

    The tests of receptive vocabulary, decoding, reading comprehension, and mathematics all suggested estimates of group heritability and full sample heritability of moderate effect sizes, and all estimates were statistically significant. Furthermore, all estimates were within confidence intervals of previously reported estimates from twin and adoption studies.

    Conclusion

    The present study provides additional support for significant genetic effects across low and wide ranges of specific achievement. Moreover, this study supports that genetic influences on reading, language, and mathematics are generalizable beyond twin and adoption studies.

    REFERENCES

    • Alarcón, M., DeFries, J. C., Light, J. G., & Pennington, B. F. (1997). A twin study of mathematics disability.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 617–623.
    • Baker, P., Keck, C., Mott, F., & Quinlan, S. (1993). NLSY child handbook (rev ed.). Columbus, OH: Center for Human Resource Research.
    • Baumrind, D. (1993). The average expectable environment is not good enough: A response to Scarr.Child Development, 64, 1134–1156.
    • Bishop, D. V. M., Bishop, S. J., Bright, P., James, C., Delaney, T., & Tallal, P. (1999). Different origin of auditory and phonological processing problems in child with language impairment: Evidence from a twin study.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 155–168.
    • Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different?.Pscyhological Bulletin, 130, 858–886.
    • Brooks, A., Fulker, D. W., & DeFries, J. C. (1990). Reading performance and general cognitive ability: A multivariate genetic analysis of twin data.Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 141–146.
    • Byrne, B., Olson, R. K., Samuelsson, S., Wadsworth, S., Corley, R., & DeFries, J. C. (2006). Genetic and environmental influences on early literacy.Journal of Research in Reading, 29(1)33–49.
    • Castles, A., Bates, T., Coltheart, M., Luciano, M., & Martin, N. G. (2006). Cognitive modelling and the behaviour genetics of reading.Journal of Research in Reading, 29(1)92–103.
    • Center for Human Resource Research. (2004). NLSY79 Children and Young Adult data users guide. Retrieved September 15, 2007, from ftp://www.nlsinfo.org/pub/usersvc/Child-Young-Adult/2004ChildYA-DataUsersGuide.pdf
    • Center for Human Resource Research. (2006). NLSY79 users guide. Retrieved September 15, 2007, from ftp://www.nlsinfo.org/pub/usersvc/NLSY79/NLSY79%202004%20User%20Guide/79text/front.htm
    • Chase-Lansdale, P. L., Mott, F. L., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Phillips, D. A. (1991). Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth: A unique research opportunity.Developmental Psychology, 27(6)918–931.
    • DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1985). Multiple regression of twin data.Behavior Genetics, 15(5)467–473.
    • DeThorne, L. S., Petrill, S. A., Hart, S. A., Channell, R., Campbell, R., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2008). Estimates of genetic and environmental effects on children’s conversational language measures.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 423–435.
    • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). PPVT–R revised manual. Circle Pines, MN: AGS.
    • Dunn, L. M., & Markwardt, F. C. (1970). Examiner’s manual: Peabody Individual Achievement Test. Circle Pines, MN: AGS.
    • Eaves, L., Foley, D., & Silberg, J. (2003). Has the “equal environments” assumption been tested in twin studies?.Twin Research, 6(6)486–489.
    • Evans, D. M., & Martin, M. G. (2000). The validity of twin studies.GeneScreen, 1, 77–79.
    • Flynn, J. R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains 1932 to 1978.Psychological Bulletin, 95, 29–51.
    • Foch, T. T., & Plomin, R. (1980). Specific cognitive abilities in 5- to 12-year-old twins.Behavior Genetics, 10(6)507–520.
    • Gayan, J., & Olson, R. K. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in printed word recognition.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84(2)97–123.
    • Harlaar, N., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2007). Reading exposure: A (largely) environmental risk factor with environmentally-mediated effects on reading performance in the primary school years.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(12)1192–1199.
    • Harlaar, N., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., & Plomin, R. (2005). Reading and general cognitive ability: A multivariate analysis of 7-year-old twins.Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(3)197–218.
    • Hart, S. A., Petrill, S. A., Thompson, L. A., & Plomin, R. (2009). The ABCs of math: A genetic analysis of mathematics and its links with reading ability and general cognitive ability.Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2)388–402.
    • Hawke, J. L., Wadsworth, S. J., Olson, R. K., & Defries, J. C. (2007). Etiology of reading difficulties as a function of gender and severity.Reading and Writing, 20(1–2)13–25.
    • Knopik, V. S., & DeFries, J. C. (1999). Etiology of covariation between reading and mathematics performance: A twin study.Twin Research, 2, 226–234.
    • Kovas, Y., & Plomin, R. (2006). Generalist genes: Implications for the cognitive sciences.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5)198–203.
    • LaBuda, M. C., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1986). Multiple regression analysis of twin data obtained from selected samples.Genetic Epidemiology, 3(6)425–433.
    • Light, J. G., & DeFries, J. C. (1995). Comorbidity of reading and mathematics disabilities: Genetic and environmental etiologies.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 96–106.
    • Light, J. G., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (1998). Multivariate behavioral genetic analysis of achievement and cognitive measures in reading disabled and control twin pairs.Human Biology, 70, 215–237.
    • Oliver, B., Harlaar, N., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Kovas, Y., Walker, S. O., & Petrill, S. A. (2004). A twin study of teacher-reported mathematics performance and low performance in 7-year-olds.Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 504–517.
    • Petrill, S. A., Deater-Deckard, K., Thompson, L. A., DeThorne, L. S., & Schatschneider, C. (2006). Reading skills in early readers: Genetic AND shared environmental influences.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(1)48–55.
    • Petrill, S. A., Johansson, B., Pederson, N. L., Berg, S., Plomin, R., & Ahern, F. (2001). Low cognitive functioning in nondemented 80+-year-old twins is not heritable.Intelligence, 29, 75–83.
    • Plomin, R., & Kovas, Y. (2005). Generalist genes and learning disabilities.Psychological Bulletin, 131(4)592–617.
    • Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & McGuffin, P. (2008). Behavioral genetics (4th ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman.
    • Plomin, R., & McGuffin, P. (2003). Psychopathology in the postgenomic era.Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 205–228.
    • Plomin, R., & Rende, R. (1991). Human behavioral genetics.In M. R. Rosenzweig, & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (pp. 161–190). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
    • Rodgers, J. L., Johnson, A. B., & Bard, D. E. (2005). NLSY—children/young adult (1986–2000) kinship linking algorithm. Unpublished manuscript, University of Oklahoma, Norman.
    • Rodgers, J. L., & Kohler, H. (2005). Reformulating and simplifying the DF analysis model.Behavior Genetics, 35(2)211–217.
    • Rodgers, J. L., & McGue, M. (1994). A simple algebraic demonstration of the validity of DeFries-Fulker analysis in unselected samples with multiple kinship levels.Behavior Genetics, 24(2)259–262.
    • Rodgers, J. L., & Rowe, D. (1987). IQ similarity in twins, siblings, half-siblings, cousins and random pairs.Intelligence, 11, 199–206.
    • Rodgers, J. L., Rowe, D., & Li, C. (1994). Beyond nature vs. nurture: DF analyses of nonshared influences on problem behaviors.Developmental Psychology, 30, 374–384.
    • Rodgers, J. L., Rowe, D., & May, K. (1994). DF analysis of NLSY IQ/achievement data: Nonshared environmental influences.Intelligence, 19, 157–177.
    • Rodgers, J. L., & Wänström, L. (2007). Identification of a Flynn effect in the NLSY: Moving from the center to the boundaries.Intelligence, 35, 187–196.
    • Stoolmiller, M. (1998). Correcting estimates of shared environmental variance for range restriction in adoption studies using a truncated multivariate normal model.Behavior Genetics, 28(6)429–441.
    • Stoolmiller, M. (1999). Implications of the restricted range of family environments for estimates of heritability and nonshared environment in behavior-genetic adoption designs.Psychological Bulletin, 125(4)392–409.
    • U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2007). NLS Web-Investigator, 2007 [Data file]. Available from http://www.bls.gov/nls/nlsdata.htm
    • U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2009). National Longitudinal Surveys. Retrieved November 24, 2009, from http://www.bls.gov/nls/publications.htm
    • Van Hulle, C. A., Rodgers, J. L., D’Onofrio, B. M., Waldman, I. D., & Lahey, B. B. (2007). Sex differences in the causes of self-reported adolescent delinquency.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(2)236–248.
    • Viding, E., Spinath, F. M., Price, T. S., Bishop, D. V. M., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2004). Genetic and environmental influence on language impairment in 4-year-old same-sex and opposite-sex twins.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 315–325.
    • Wadsworth, S. J., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, J. K., & DeFries, J. C. (2001). Stability of genetic and environmental influences on reading performance at 7, 12, and 16 years of age in the Colorado Adoption Project.Behavior Genetics, 31(4)353–359.

    Additional Resources