Abstract
Purpose
This study compared the development of essential elements of narrative skill in children from African American English (AAE)- and general American English (GAE)-speaking communities using an innovative elicitation and evaluation protocol consisting of four key indices of narrative language: (a) reference contrasting, (b) temporal expressions, (c) mental state descriptions, and (d) understanding of behavior based on false belief.
Method
Participants were 291 AAE speakers and 238 GAE speakers, 4 to 9 years of age. Approximately one-third of both dialect groups were identified as having language impairments. Children generated 2 stories based on short picture sequences. Their stories were coded for the 4 key indices of narrative language. Analyses of variance were performed with subsets of the measures and a composite index with all measures combined as outcomes; and with age, dialect group, and clinical status as predictors.
Results
Age and clinical status had statistically significant effects on the subset measures and the composite score. Variation between AAE and GAE dialect was not a significant factor.
Conclusion
By focusing on dialect-neutral elements of narratives—creating links across sentences and providing mental state interpretations—this study adds to our knowledge of development and impairment in narrative production among both AAE- and GAE-background children.
REFERENCES
-
Allen, B. A., de Villiers, J. G., & François, S. (2001). Deficits versus differences: African American children’s linguistic paths towards a theory of mind.InM. Almgren, A. Barrena, M.-J. Ezeizabarrena, I. Idiazabal, & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Research on child language acquisition: Proceedings of the 8th Conference of the International Association for the Study of Child Language (pp. 1006–1014). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. - American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2005). Annual counts of the ASHA membership and affiliation, year-end 2005. Retrieved from http://www.asha.org/uploadedfiles/research/05wwAethnicityrace
-
Astington, J. (1993). The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. -
Bamberg, M. (1986). A functional approach to the acquisition of anaphoric relationships.Linguistics, 24, 227–284. -
Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children’s narrative competencies.Journal of Child Language, 18, 689–710. -
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?.Cognition, 21, 37–46. -
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1989). Young children’s attribution of action to beliefs and desires.Child Development, 60, 946–964. -
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. -
Berman, R. A. (1988). On the ability to relate events in narrative.Discourse Processes, 11, 469–497. -
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A cross-linguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. -
Bliss, L. S., Covington, Z., & McCabe, A. (1999). Assessing the narratives of African American children.Contemporary Issues in Communication Science and Disorders, 26, 160–167. -
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. -
Bruner, J., & Feldman, C. (1993). Theories of mind and the problem of autism.InS. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 267–291). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. -
Burns, F. A. (2004). Elicited versus open-ended narratives in African American children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. -
Campbell, J. (1949). The hero with a thousand faces. New York, NY: Pantheon Books. -
Champion, T. (1997). “Tell me somethin' good”: A description of narrative structures among African American children.Linguistics and Education, 9, 251–286. -
Champion, T. (2003). Understanding storytelling among African American children: A journey from Africa to America. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. -
Champion, T., Seymour, H., & Camarata, S. (1995). Narrative discourse of African American children.Journal of Narrative and Life History, 5, 333–352. -
Charity, A. (2007). Regional differences in low-SES African American children’s speech in the school setting.Language Variation and Change, 19, 281–293. -
Crago, M. B., Eriks-Brophy, A., Pesco, D., & McAlpine, L. (1997). Culturally based miscommunication in classroom interaction.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 28, 245–254. -
Curenton, S. M., & Justice, L. M. (2004). African American and Caucasian preschoolers' use of decontextualized language: Literate language features in oral narratives.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 240–253. -
Dasinger, L., & Toupin, C. (1994). The development of relative clause functions in narrative.InR. A. Berman, & D. I. Slobin (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: A cross-linguistic developmental study (pp. 457–514). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. -
de Villiers, J., & Pyers, J. (1997). Complementing cognition: The relationship between language and theory of mind.InE. Hughes, M. Hughes, & A. Greenhill (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 136–147). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. -
de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding.Cognitive Development, 17, 1037–1060. -
de Villiers, J. G., Roeper, T., Bland-Stewart, L., & Pearson, B. Z. (2008). Answering hard questions: Wh- movement across dialects and disorder.Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 67–103. -
de Villiers, P. A. (2004). Assessing pragmatic skills in elicited production.Seminars in Speech and Language, 25(1)57–72. -
de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., Picone, C., Wilkins, A., Dinkins, E., & Burns, F. (2010). Dialect and narrative skills in African American preschoolers.InK. Franich, K. M. Iserman, & L. L. Keil (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 114–125). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. -
Fiestas, C. E., & Peña, E. D. (2004). Narrative discourse in bilingual children: Language and task effects.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 155–168. -
Gee, J. P. (1985). The narrativization of experience in the oral style.Journal of Education, 167, 9–35. -
Gee, J. P. (1986). Units in the production of narrative discourse.Discourse Processes, 9, 391–422. -
Gillam, R., & Pearson, N. (2004). Test of Narrative Language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. -
Gorman, B. K., Fiestas, C. E., Pena, E. D., & Clark, M. R. (2011). Creative and stylistic devices employed by children during a storybook narrative task: A cross-cultural study.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 42, 167–181. -
Green, L. J. (2008, November). Literacy and developing African American English-speaking children.Invited symposium at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Chicago, IL . -
Greenhalgh, K. S., & Strong, C. J. (2001). Literate language features in spoken narratives of children with typical language and children with language impairments.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 114–125. -
Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F., & Quinn, R. (1993). Assessing narratives of children from diverse cultural/linguistic groups.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 24, 2–9. -
Hall, W. S. (1984). African American English corpus. Retrieved from http://childes.psy.cmu.edu/data/Eng-USA -
Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London, UK: Longman. -
Harris, P. (1996). Desires, beliefs, and language.InP. Carruthers, & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 200–222). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. -
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. -
Hester, E. J. (1996). Narratives of young African American children.InA. Kamhi, K. Pollock, & J. Harris (Eds.), Communication development and disorders in African American children (pp. 227–245). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. -
Hickmann, M. (2003). Children’s discourse: Person, space, and time across languages. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. -
Holmes, H. A., Black, C., & Miller, S. A. (1996). A cross-task comparison of false belief understanding in a Head Start population.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 263–285. -
Horton-Ikard, R. (2009). Cohesive adequacy in the narrative samples of school-age children who use African American English.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40, 393–402. -
Hyon, S., & Sulzby, E. (1994). African American kindergartners' spoken narratives: Topic associating and topic centered styles.Linguistics and Education, 6, 121–152. -
Justice, L. M., Bowles, R., Kaderavek, J. K., Ukrainetz, T., Eisenberg, S., & Gillam, R. (2006). The Index of Narrative Micro-Structure (INMIS): A clinical tool for analyzing school-aged children’s narrative performance.American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15, 1–15. -
Justice, L. M., Bowles, R., Pence, K., & Gosse, C. (2010). A scalable tool for assessing children’s language abilities within a narrative context: The NAP.Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 218–234. -
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city: Studies of Black English vernacular. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. -
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. -
Liles, B. Z. (1985). Cohesion in the narratives of normal and language disordered children.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 123–133. -
Liles, B. Z. (1987). Episode organization and cohesive conjunctives in narratives of children with and without language disorder.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 185–196. -
Mandler, J. M. (1982). Some uses and abuses of a story grammar.Discourse Processes, 5, 305–318. -
Matsuyama, U. K. (1983). Can story grammar speak Japanese?.The Reading Teacher, 36, 666–669. -
Mayer, M. (1967). A boy, a dog, and a frog. New York, NY: Dial Books. -
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you?. New York, NY: Dial Books. -
Mayer, M. (1974). Frog goes to dinner. New York, NY: Dial Books. -
Mayer, M., & Mayer, M. (1975). One frog too many. New York, NY: Dial Books. -
Melzi, G. (2000). Cultural variations in the construction of personal narratives: Central American and European American mothers' elicitation styles.Discourse Processes, 30, 153–177. -
Michaels, S. (1981). Sharing time: Children’s narrative style and differential access to literacy.Language and Society, 10, 423–442. -
Michaels, S. (1986). Narrative presentations: An oral preparation for literacy with first graders.InJ. Cook-Gumperz (Ed.), The social construction of literacy (pp. 94–116). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. -
Miller, L., Gillam, R., & Peña, E. (2003). Dynamic assessment and intervention: Improving children’s narrative abilities. Circle Pines, MN: AGS. -
Pankratz, M. E., Plante, E., Vance, R., & Insalaco, D. M. (2007). The diagnostic and predictive validity of the Renfrew Bus Story.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 390–399. -
Pearson, B. Z. (2001). Logic and mind in Spanish–English children’s narratives.InL. Verhoeven, & S. Stromqvist (Eds.), Narrative development in a multilingual context (pp. 373–398). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. -
Pearson, B. Z. (2002). Narrative competence among monolingual and bilingual school children in Miami.InD. K. Oller, & R. E. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 135–174). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. -
Pearson, B. Z., & Ciolli, L. (2004). Distinguishing dialect and development from disorder: Case studies.Seminars in Speech and Language, 25(1)101–112. -
Pearson, B. Z., Velleman, S. L., Bryant, T. J., & Charko, T. (2009). Phonological milestones for African American English speaking children learning mainstream American English as a second dialect.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40, 229–244. -
Pellegrini, A. D. (1984). The effect of dramatic play on children’s generation of cohesive text.Discourse Processes, 7, 57–67. -
Perera, K. (1986). Language acquisition and writing.InP. Fletcher, & M. Garman (Eds.), Language acquisition (pp. 494–518). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. -
Perner, J., Leekam, S., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137. -
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York, NY: Plenum. -
Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2000). Insights into theory of mind from deafness and autism.Mind & Language, 15, 123–145. -
Schneider, P., Dube, R. V., & Hayward, D. (2005). The Edmonton Narrative Norms Instrument. Retrieved from http://www.rehabmed.ualberta.ca/spa/enni -
Schraeder, T., Quinn, M., Stockman, I. J., & Miller, J. (1999). Authentic assessment as an approach to preschool speech-language screening.American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 6, 195–200. -
Seymour, H. N., Roeper, T., & de Villiers, J. G. (2000). Dialect Sensitive Language Test (Unpublished pilot edition). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. -
Seymour, H. N., Roeper, T., & de Villiers, J. G. (2003). Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variation—Screening Test. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. -
Seymour, H. N., Roeper, T., & de Villiers, J. G. (2005). Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variation—Norm Referenced. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. -
Snow, C., Tabors, P., Nicholson, P., & Kurland, B. (1995). SHELL: Oral language and early literacy skills in kindergarten and first grade.Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 10, 37–48. -
Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children.InR. O. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp. 53–120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. -
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., … Levinson, S. C. (2009). Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(26)10587–10592. -
Strong, C. J. (1998). The Strong Narrative Assessment Procedure. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications. -
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison of narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals.Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 241–256. -
Ukrainetz, T. A., Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., Eisenberg, S. L., Gillam, R. B., & Harm, H. M. (2005). The development of expressive elaboration in fictional narratives.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1363–1377. - United States Bureau of the Census. (2000). Current population survey: School enrollment supplemental file [CD ROM]. Washington DC: Author.
-
Washington, J. A., & Craig, H. K. (1998). Socioeconomic status and gender influences on children’s dialectal variations.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 41, 618–626. -
Wellman, H. M., & Bartsch, K. (1994). Before belief: Children’s early psychological theory.InC. Lewis, & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children’s early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 331–354). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. -
Westby, C. E. (1991). Learning to talk, talking to learn: Oral–literate language differences.InC. Simon (Ed.), Communication skills and classroom success: Assessment and therapy methodologies for language and learning disabled students (pp. 181–218). San Diego, CA: College Hill Press. -
Westby, C. E. (1999). Assessing and facilitating text comprehension problems.InH. W. Catts, & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (pp. 154–221). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. -
Wolfram, W. (1991). Dialects and American English. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.