In this article, I share my thoughts concerning what children with developmental language disorders should be called, how they should be defined, and how we might differentiate children with specific language impairment (SLI) from other children with developmental language disorders. Among other things, I attempt to show why a lack of congruence between clinical and research constructs should be expected.
Researchers and clinicians use different identification criterion and procedures because clinical and educational objectives are different from research objectives. While recognizing these differences, I suggest several possible ways to differentiate a subgroup of children with SLI from the general population of children with developmental language disorders without using nonverbal IQ. Even if researchers are able to identify this unique group of children, clinicians may never embrace the SLI construct.
In the best of all possible worlds, clinicians would be familiar with how researchers define SLI and appreciate the value of research that attempts to identify distinct subgroups of children with developmental language disorders. Researchers, in this ideal world, would recognize and acknowledge the lack of congruence between the research populations of SLI and the larger clinical population of children with developmental language disorders.
REFERENCES
- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1989). Issues in determining eligibility for language intervention.Asha, 31, 113–118.
- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Committee on Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools. (1980, March). Definitions for communicative disorders and differences.Asha, 22, 317–318.
-
Aram, D. M., MorrisR., & Hall, N. E. (1993). Clinical and research congruence in identifying children with specific language impairment.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 580–591. -
Aram, D. M., & Nation, J. E. (1982). Child language disorders. St. Louis, MO: C.V. Mosby -
Arthur, G. (1952). The Arthur adaptation of the Leiter International Performance Scale. Washington, DC: Psychological Service Center Press. -
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1977). From gesture to first word: On cognitive and social prerequisites.InM. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language (pp. 247–307). New York: John Wiley & Sons. -
Bishop, D. V. M. (1992). The underlying nature of specific language impairment.Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 3–66. -
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (1988). Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. -
Burgemeister, B., Blum, L., & Lorge, I. (1972). Columbia Mental Maturity Scale (3rd ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation. -
Casby, M. W. (1992). The cognitive hypothesis and its influence on speech-language services in schools.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 23, 198–202. -
Catts, H. (1993). The relationship between speech-language impairments and reading disabilities.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 948–958. -
Cole, K. (1996). What is the evidence from research with young children with language disorders?.ASHA Division 1 Language Learning and Education Newsletter, 3, 6–7. -
Cole, K. N., Dale, P. S., & Mills, P. E. (1990). Defining language delay in young children by cognitive referencing: Are we saying more than we know?.Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 291–302. -
Cole, K. N., Dale, P. S., & Mills, P. E. (1992). Stability of the intelligence quotient—language quotient relation: Is discrepancy modeling based on a myth?.American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97(2), 131–143. -
Cole, K., Schwartz, I., Notari, A., Dale, P., & Mills, P. (1995). Examination of the stability of two measures of defining specific language impairment.Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 103–123. -
Craig, H., & Evans, J. (1989). Turn exchange characteristics of SLI children’s simultaneous and nonsimultaneous speech.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 334–347. -
Craig, H., & Evans, J. (1993). Pragmatics and SLI: Within-group variations in discourse behaviors.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 777–790. -
Craig, H., & Washington, J. (1993). Access behaviors of children with specific language impairment.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 311–321. -
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test– III. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. -
Dunn, M., Flax, J., Sliwinski, M., & Aram, D. (1996). The use of spontaneous language measures as criteria for identifying children with specific language impairment: An attempt to reconcile clinical and research incongruence.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 643–654. -
Fey, M. (1996). Cognitive referencing in the study of children with language impairments.ASHA Division 1 Language Learning and Education Newsletter, 3, 7–8. -
Fey, M. E., Long, S. H., & Cleave, P. L. (1994). Reconsideration of IQ criteria in the definition of specific language impairment.InR. V. Watkins & M. L. Rice (Eds.), Specific language impairments in children (pp. 161–178). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. -
Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection?.Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. -
Johnston, J. R. (1988). Specific language disorders in the child.InN. J. Lass (Ed.), Handbook of speech-language pathology and audiology (pp. 685–715). St. Louis, MO: C.V. Mosby. -
Kamhi, A. (1996). Linguistic and cognitive aspects of specific language impairment.InM. D. Smith & J. S. Damico (Eds.), Childhood language disorders (pp. 97–118). New York: Thieme. -
Kamhi, A., & Catts, H. (1989). Reading disabilities: A developmental language perspective. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. -
Kamhi, A., & Johnston, J. (1982). Toward an understanding of retarded children’s linguistic deficiencies.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 177–184. -
Kiernan, B., Snow, D., Swisher, L., & Vance, R. (1997). Another look at nonverbal rule induction in children with SLI: Testing a flexible reconceptualization hypothesis.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 75–83. -
Lahey, M. (1990). Who shall be called language disordered? Some reflections and one perspective.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 612–620. -
Leonard, L. (1990). Early language development and language disorders.InG. Shames & E. Wiig (Eds.), Human communication disorders: An introduction (4th ed., pp. 174–212). Columbus, OH: Merrill. -
Leonard, L. (1994). Some problems facing accounts of morphological deficits in children with specific language impairments.InR. Watkins & M. Rice (Eds.), Specific language impairments in children (pp. 91–107). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. -
Leonard, L. (1995). Functional categories in the grammars of children with specific language impairment.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 1270–1283. -
Lyon, G. (1995). Toward a definition of dyslexia.Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 3–26. -
McFadden, T. U. (1996). Creating language impairments in typically achieving children: The pitfalls of “normal” normative sampling.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 27, 3–9. -
Merzenich, M., Jenkins, W., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S., & Tallal, P. (1996). Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training.Science, 271, 77–81. -
Newcomer, P., & Hammill, D. (1997). Test of Language Develop-ment–P:3. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. -
Rice, M. (1994). Grammatical categories of children with specific language impairments.InR. Watkins & M. Rice (Eds.), Specific language impairments in children (pp. 69–91). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. -
Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1995). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamental–3. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. -
Stanovich, K., & Siegel, L. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model.Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53. -
Stark, R., & Tallal, P. (1981). Selection of children with specific language deficits.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 46, 114–122. -
Tallal, P., Miller, S., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W., & Merzenich, M. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech.Science, 271, 81–84. -
Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973). Developmental aphasia: Impaired rate of non-verbal processing as a function of sensory modality.Neuropsychologia, 11, 389–398. -
Tomblin, B. (1996). The big picture of SLI: Results of an epidemiologic study of SLI among kindergarten children.Paper presented at the Symposium for Research in Child Language Disorders ,Madison, WI . -
Tomblin, B., Records, N., & Zhang, X. (1996). A system for the diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten children.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1284–1294. -
Wallach, G. P., & Butler, K. G. (1994). Language learning disabilities in school-age children and adolescents. New York: Merrill. -
Wechsler, D. (1967). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. New York: Psychological Corp. -
Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised. New York: Psychological Corp.