No AccessJournal of Speech, Language, and Hearing ResearchResearch Article1 Aug 2006

Perception of Coarticulatory Information in Normal Speech and Dysarthria

    Purpose

    This study addressed three research questions: (a) Can listeners use anticipatory vowel information in prevocalic consonants produced by talkers with dysarthria to identify the upcoming vowel? (b) Are listeners sensitive to interspeaker variation in anticipatory coarticulation during prevocalic consonants produced by healthy talkers and/or talkers with dysarthria, as measured by vowel identification accuracy? (c) Is interspeaker variation in anticipatory coarticulation reflected in measures of intelligibility?

    Method

    Stimuli included 106 CVC words produced by 20 speakers with either Parkinson’s disease or multiple sclerosis or by 16 healthy controls characterized by an operationally defined normal, under, or over level of anticipatory vowel coarticulation. Ten listeners were presented with prevocalic consonants for identification of the vowel. Ten additional listeners judged single-word intelligibility. An analysis of variance was used to determine differences in vowel identification accuracy and intelligibility as a function of speaker group, coarticulation level, and vowel type.

    Results

    Listeners accurately identified vowels produced by all speaker groups from the aperiodic portion of prevocalic consonants, but interspeaker variations in strength of coarticulation did not strongly affect vowel identification accuracy or intelligibility.

    Conclusions

    Listeners appear to be tuned to similar types of information in the acoustic speech stream irrespective of the source or speaker, and any perceptual effects of interspeaker variation in coarticulation are subtle.

    References

    • Ackermann, H., & Ziegler, W. (1991). Articulatory deficits in Parkinsonian dysarthria: An acoustic analysis.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 54, 1093–1098.
    • Baum, S. R. (1998). Anticipatory coarticulation in aphasia: Effects of utterance complexity.Brain and Language, 63, 357–380.
    • Beddor, P. S., Harnsberger, J. D., & Lindemann, S. (2002). Language-specific patterns of vowel-to-vowel coarticulation: Acoustic structures and their perceptual correlates.Journal of Phonetics, 30, 591–627.
    • Elman, J. L., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). Exploiting lawful variability in the speech waveform.In J. S. Perkell, & D. H. Klatt (Eds.), Invariance and variability in speech processing (pp. 360–385). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.
    • Gaskell, M. G., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1997). Integrating form and meaning in a distributed model of speech perception.Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 613–656.
    • Gaskell, M. G., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1999). Ambiguity, competition, and blending in spoken word recognition.Cognitive Science, 23, 439–432.
    • Goldinger, S. D., Pisoni, D. B., & Luce, P. A. (1996). Speech perception and spoken word recognition: Research and theory.In N. J. Lass (Ed.), Principles of experimental phonetics (pp. 277–327). St. Louis, MO: Mosby-Year Book.
    • Hertrich, I., & Ackermann, H. (1999). Temporal and spectral aspects of coarticulation in ataxic dysarthria: An acoustic analysis.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 367–381.
    • Katz, W. F. (1988). Anticipatory coarticulation in aphasia: Acoustic and perceptual data.Brain and Language, 35, 340–368.
    • Katz, W. F. (2000). Anticipatory coarticulation and aphasia: Implications for phonetic theories.Journal of Phonetics, 28, 313–334.
    • Katz, W. F., Kripke, C., & Tallal, P. (1991). Anticipatory coarticulation in the speech of adults and young children: Acoustic, perceptual, and video data.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 1222–1232.
    • Kent, R., & Moll, K. (1972). Cinefluorographic analyses of selected lingual consonants.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 15, 453–473.
    • Kuehn, D. P., & Moll, K. L. (1972). Perceptual effects of forward coarticulation.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 15, 654–664.
    • Kuhnert, B., & Hoole, P. (2004). Speaker-specific kinematic properties of alveolar reductions in English and German.Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 18, 559–575.
    • Liberman, A. M., & Mattingly, I. G. (1989). A specialization for speech perception.Science, 243, 489–494.
    • Lubker, J. F., & Gay, T. (1982). Anticipatory labial coarticulation: Experimental, biological and linguistic.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 71, 437–448.
    • Luce, P. A., Goldinger, S. D., Auer, E. T., & Vitevitch, M. S. (2000). Phonetic priming, neighborhood activation, and PARSYN.Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 615–625.
    • Luce, P., & McClellan, C. (2005). Spoken word recognition: The challenge of variation.In D. B. Pisoni, & R. B. Remez (Eds.), The handbook of speech perception (pp. 591–609). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
    • Manuel, S. Y. (1987). Acoustic and perceptual consequences of vowel-to-vowel coarticulation in three Bantu languages. Unpublished dissertation, Yale University
    • Martin, J. G., & Bunnell, H. T. (1981). Perception of anticipatory coarticulation effects.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 69, 559–567.
    • Martin, J. G., & Bunnell, H. T. (1982). Perception of anticipatory coarticulation effects in vowel–stop consonant–vowel sequences.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 473–488.
    • Mattys, S. (2004). Stress versus coarticulation: Toward an integrated approach to explicit speech segmentation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 397–408.
    • McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception.Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–86.
    • McQueen, J., & Cutler, A. (1997). Cognitive processes in speech perception.In W. J. Hardcastle, & J. Laver (Eds.), The handbook of phonetic sciences (pp. 566–586). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
    • Milenkovic, P. (1997). CSpeechSp 4.0 [Computer software].Madison, WI: Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Wisconsin.
    • Nittrouer, S. (1995). Children learn separate aspects of speech production at different rates: Evidence from spectral moments.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 520–530.
    • Nittrouer, S., & Whalen, D. H. (1989). The perceptual effects of child–adult differences in fricative–vowel coarticulation.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 86, 1266–1276.
    • Nittrouer, S., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & McGowan, R. S. (1989). The emergence of phonetic segments: Evidence from the spectral structure of fricative–vowel syllables spoken by children and adults.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 120–132.
    • Ostreicher, H. J., & Sharf, D. J. (1976). Effects of coarticulation on the identification of deleted consonant and vowel sounds.Journal of Phonetics, 4, 285–301.
    • Sereno, J. A., Baum, S. R., Marean, G. C., & Lieberman, P. (1987). Acoustic analysis and perceptual data on anticipatory labial coarticulation in adults and children.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81, 512–519.
    • Southwood, H., Dagenais, P. A., Sutphin, S. M., & Garcia, J. M. (1997). Coarticulation in apraxia of speech: A perceptual, acoustic, and electropalatographic study.Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 11, 179–203.
    • Sussman, H. M., Fruchter, D., Hilbert, J., & Sirosh, J. (1998). Linear correlates in the speech signal: The orderly output constraint.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 241–299.
    • Tjaden, K. (2003). Anticipatory coarticulation in multiple sclerosis and Parkinson’s disease.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 990–1008.
    • Tjaden, K., & Turner, G. (1997). Spectral properties of fricatives in ALS.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1358–1372.
    • Tjaden, K., & Wilding, G. E. (2004). Rate and loudness manipulations in dysarthria: Acoustic and perceptual findings.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 766–783.
    • Tjaden, K., & Wilding, G. E. (2005). Effect of rate reduction and increased loudness on acoustic measures of anticipatory coarticulation in multiple sclerosis and Parkinson disease.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 261–277.
    • Waldstein, R. S., & Baum, S. R. (1994). Perception of coarticulatory cues in the speech of children with profound hearing loss and children with normal hearing.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 952–959.
    • Weismer, G., & Martin, R. (1992). Acoustic and perceptual approaches to the study of intelligibility.In R. Kent (Ed.), Intelligibility in speech disorders: Theory, measurement, and management (pp. 68–118). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
    • Weismer, G., Yunusova, Y., & Westbury, J. R. (2003). Interarticulator coordination in dysarthria: An X-ray microbeam study.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1247–1261.
    • Winitz, H., Scheib, M., & Reeds, J. (1972). Identification of stops and vowels from the burst portion of /p t k/ isolated from conversational speech.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 51, 1309–1317.
    • Yeni-Komshian, G. H., & Soli, S. D. (1981). Recognition of vowels from information in fricatives: Perceptual evidence of fricative–vowel coarticulation.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 70, 966–975.
    • Yorkston, K., & Beukelman, D. (1981). Assessment of the intelligibility of dysarthric speech. Tigard, OR: CC Publications.
    • Ziegler, W., & von Cramon, D. R. (1985). Anticipatory coarticulation in a patient with apraxia of speech.Brain and Language, 26, 117–130.
    • Ziegler, W., & von Cramon, D. R. (1986). Spastic dysarthria after acquired brain injury: An acoustic study.British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 21, 173–187.

    Additional Resources