Purpose

The purpose of this study was to compare speech production at 12 months of age for children with hearing loss (HL) who were identified and received intervention before 6 months of age with those of children with normal hearing (NH).

Method

The speech production of 10 children with NH was compared with that of 10 children with HL whose losses were identified (better ear pure-tone average at 0.5, 1, and 2 kHz poorer than 50 dB HL) and whose intervention started before 6 months of age. These children were recorded at 12 months of age interacting with a parent. Three properties of speech production were analyzed: (a) syllable shape, (b) consonant type, and (c) vowel formant frequencies.

Results

Children with HL had (a) fewer multisyllable utterances with consonants, (b) fewer fricatives and fewer stops with alveolar-velar stop place, and (c) more restricted front-back tongue positions for vowels than did the children with NH.

Conclusion

Even when hearing loss is identified shortly after birth, children with HL do not develop speech production skills as their peers with NH do at 12 months of age. This suggests that researchers need to consider their approaches to early intervention carefully.

References

  • Boysson-Bardies, B., Sagart, L., Halle, P., & Durand, C. (1986). Acoustic investigations of cross-linguistic variability in babbling.In B. Lindblom, & R. Zetterstrom (Eds.), Precursors of early speech (pp. 113–126). New York: Stockton Press.
  • Boysson-Bardies, B., Halle, P., Sagart, L., & Durand, C. (1989). A cross-linguistic investigation of vowel formants in babbling.Journal of Child Language, 16, 1–17.
  • Buhr, R. D. (1980). The emergence of vowels in an infant.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 23, 73–94.
  • Byrne, D., Dillon, H., Tran, K., Arlinger, S., Wilbraham, K., & Cox, R. (1994). An international comparison of long-term average speech spectra.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96, 2108–2120.
  • Commission on Education of the Deaf. (1988). Toward equality: Education of the deaf. A report to the President and Congress of the United States. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education and Bureau of Maternal and Child Health.
  • Davis, B. L., & MacNeilage, P. F. (1990). Acquisition of correct vowel production: A quantitative case study.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 16–27.
  • Davis, B. L., & MacNeilage, P. F. (1995). The articulatory basis of babbling.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 1199–1211.
  • Hollingshead, A. (1965). Two factor index of social position. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Kent, R. D. (1996). Hearing and believing: Some limits to the auditory-perceptual assessment of speech and voice disorders.American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 5, 7–23.
  • Kent, R. D., & Bauer, H. R. (1985). Vocalizations of one-year-olds.Journal of Child Language, 12, 491–526.
  • Kent, R. D., & Murray, A. D. (1982). Acoustic features of infant vocalic utterances at 3, 6, and 9 months.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 72, 353–365.
  • Kent, R. D., Osberger, M. J., Netsell, R., & Hustedde, C. G. (1987). Phonetic development of identical twins differing in auditory function.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 64–75.
  • Koopmans-van Beinum, F. J., & Doppen, L. (2003). Development of deaf and hearing infants.Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. 1033–1066. Barcelona, Spain
  • Moeller, M. P. (2000). Early intervention and language development in children who are deaf and hard of hearing.Pediatrics, 106, E43.
  • Nittrouer, S. (in press). Development outcomes in the first two years of life: Effects of early identified hearing loss and sign language.Ear and Hearing.
  • Nittrouer, S., & Burton, L. T. (2005). The role of early language experience in the development of speech perception and phonological processing abilities: Evidence from 5-year-olds with histories of otitis media with effusion and low socioeconomic status.Journal of Communication Disorders, 38, 29–63.
  • Oller, D. K. (1986). Metaphonology and infant vocalizations.In B. Lindblom, & R. Zetterstrom (Eds.), Precursors of early speech (pp. 21–36). New York: Stockton Press.
  • Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (1988). The role of audition in infant babbling.Child Development, 59, 441–449.
  • Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Bull, D. H., & Carney, A. E. (1985). Preschool vocalizations of a deaf infant: A comparison with normal metaphonological development.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 47–63.
  • Rathmann, P. (1994). Good night, gorilla. New York: Putnams.
  • Rescorla, L. (1989). The language development survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587–599.
  • Rvachew, S., Mattock, K., Polka, L., & Ménard, L. (2006). Developmental and cross-linguistic variation in the infant vowel space: The case of Canadian English and Canadian French.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 2250–2259.
  • Rvachew, S., Slawinski, E. B., Williams, M., & Green, C. L. (1996). Formant frequencies of vowels produced by infants with and without early onset otitis media.Canadian Acoustics, 24, 19–28.
  • Stoel-Gammon, C., & Otomo, K. (1986). Babbling development of hearing-impaired and normally hearing subjects.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 33–41.
  • Yoshinago-Itano, C., Coulter, D., & Thompson, V. (2001). Developmental outcomes of children with hearing loss born in Colorado hospitals with and without universal newborn hearing screening programs.Seminars in Neonatology, 6, 521–529.
  • Yoshinago-Itano, C., Sedey, A. L., Coulter, D. K., & Mehl, A. L. (1998). Language of early- and later-identified children with hearing loss.Pediatrics, 102, 1161–1171.

Additional Resources