Abstract
Purpose
The authors investigated continuity and discontinuity of vocabulary skills in a population-based cohort in the Netherlands.
Method
Mothers of 3,759 children completed the Dutch version of the MacArthur Short Form Vocabulary Checklist (Zink & Lejaegere, 2003) at 18 months and a Dutch translation of the Language Development Survey (Rescorla, 1989) at 30 months. At both ages, expressive vocabulary delay was defined as vocabulary scores <10th age- and gender-specific percentile.
Results
Of the children, 85.2% had normal vocabulary development at both ages, 6.2% were “late bloomers,” 6.0% had late onset expressive vocabulary delay, and 2.6% had persistent expressive vocabulary delay. Word production and comprehension at 18 months explained 11.5% of the variance in 30-month vocabulary scores, with low birth weight, child age, gender and ethnicity, maternal age and education, and parenting stress explaining an additional 6.2%. Multinomial logistic regression was used to identify biological, demographic, and psychological factors associated with each of the vocabulary delay outcome groups relative to the typically developing group.
Conclusions
Although multiple perinatal, demographic, and maternal psychosocial factors significantly predicted vocabulary skills at 30 months, positive predictive value and sensitivity were low. Future studies should address to what extent additional factors, such as brain maturation and genetic influences, can improve the prediction and understanding of continuity and discontinuity of language delay.
References
-
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for ASEBA Preschool Forms and Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families. -
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. -
Bishop, D. V., Price, T. S., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2003). Outcomes of early language delay: II. Etiology of transient and persistent language difficulties.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 561–575. -
Bricker, D., & Squires, J. (1999). Ages and Stages Questionnaires: A parent-completed, child-monitoring system (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. -
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. -
Dale, P. S. (1996). Parent report assessment of language and communication.InK. N. Cole, P. S. Dale, & D. J. Thal (Eds.), Assessment of communication and language (pp. 161–182). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. -
Dale, P. S., Price, T. S., Bishop, D. V., & Plomin, R. (2003). Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 544–560. -
Dale, P. S., Simonoff, E., Bishop, D. V., Eley, T. C., Oliver, B., Price, T. S., & Plomin, R. (1998). Genetic influence on language delay in two-year-old children.Nature Neuroscience, 1, 324–328. -
A. De Brock, A. A. Vermulst, J. R. M. Gerris, & R. R. Abidin (Eds.), 1992). Nijmeegse Ouderlijke Stress Index, handleiding experimentele versie [NOSIK—Nijmegen Parenting Stress Index, manual experimental version]. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets en Zeitlinger. -
Den Ouden, A. L., & Buitendijk, S. E. (2003). Vroeggeboorte samengevat [Prematurity: An overview].Retrieved from www.nationaalkompas.nl/gezondheid-en-ziekte/ziekten-en- aandoeningen/aandoeningen-perinataal /vroeggeboorten/vroeggeboorte-samengevat/ -
Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., Boivin, M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammatical development.Child Development, 74, 394–412. - Dutch Ministry of Education, Culture and Science. (2007). The education system in the Netherlands 2007. The Hague, the Netherlands: Dutch Eurydice Unit.
-
Ellis Weismer, S. (2007). Typical talkers, late talkers, and children with specific language impairment: A language endowment spectrum?.R. Paul (Ed.), The influence of developmental perspectives on research and practice in communication disorders: A festschrift for Robin S. Chapman (pp. 83–102). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. -
Feldman, H. M., Dale, P. S., Campbell, T. F., Colborn, D. K., Kurs-Lasky, M., Rockette, H. E., & Paradise, J. L. (2005). Concurrent and predictive validity of parent reports of child language at ages 2 and 3 years.Child Development, 76, 856–868. -
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., & Reilly, J. S. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. -
Gertner, B. L., Rice, M. L., & Hadley, P. A. (1994). Influence of communicative competence on peer preferences in a preschool classroom.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 913–923. -
Giddan, J. J., & Milling, L. (1999). Comorbidity of psychiatric and communication disorders in children.Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 8, 19–36. -
B. Hart, & T. Risley (Eds.), 1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. -
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech.Child Development, 74, 1368–1378. -
Horwitz, S. M., Irwin, J. R., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Bosson Heenan, J. M., Mendoza, J., & Carter, A. S. (2003). Language delay in a community cohort of young children.Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 932–940. -
Jaddoe, V. W., van Duijn, C. M., van der Heijden, A. J., Mackenbach, J. P., Moll, H. A., Steegers, E. A., & Hofman, A. (2008). The Generation R Study: Design and cohort update until the age of 4 years.European Journal of Epidemiology, 23, 801–811. -
Leonard, L. B. (2009). Is expressive language disorder an accurate diagnostic category?.American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 18, 115–123. -
Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A., Laakso, M., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Language development and symbolic play in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 873–885. -
Magill-Evans, J., & Harrison, M. J. (1999). Parent–child interactions and development of toddlers born preterm.West Journal of Nursing Research, 21, 292–307. -
Noel, M., Peterson, C., & Jesso, B. (2008). The relationship of parenting stress and child temperament to language development among economically disadvantaged preschoolers.Journal of Child Language, 35, 823–843. -
Oliver, B., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2004). Verbal and nonverbal predictors of early language problems: An analysis of twins in early childhood back to infancy.Journal of Child Language, 31, 609–631. - Perinatal Registration Netherlands. (2004). Perinatale zorg in Nederland 2004 [Perinatal care in the Netherlands in 2004]. Bilthoven, the Netherlands: Stichting Perinatale Registratie Nederland.
-
Pujol, J., Soriano-Mas, C., Ortiz, H., Sebastian-Galles, N., Losilla, J. M., & Deus, J. (2006). Myelination of language-related areas in the developing brain.Neurology, 66, 339–343. -
Reilly, S., Wake, M., Bavin, E. L., Prior, M., Williams, J., Bretherton, L., & Ukoumunne, O. C. (2007). Predicting language at 2 years of age: A prospective community study.Pediatrics, 120, e1441–e1449. -
Rescorla, L. (1989). The Language Development Survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587–599. -
Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in late-talking toddlers.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 360–371. -
Rescorla, L. (2009). Age 17 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers: Support for a dimensional perspective on language delay.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 16–30. -
Rescorla, L., & Achenbach, T. M. (2002). Use of the Language Development Survey (LDS) in a national probability sample of children 18 to 35 months old.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 733–743. -
Rescorla, L., & Alley, A. (2001). Validation of the Language Development Survey (LDS): A parent report tool for identifying language delay in toddlers.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 434–445. -
Rescorla, L., Ratner, N. B., Jusczyk, P., & Jusczyk, A. M. (2005). Concurrent validity of the Language Development Survey: Associations with the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories: Words and sentences.American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14, 156–163. -
Rescorla, L., Ross, G. S., & McClure, S. (2007). Language delay and behavioral/emotional problems in toddlers: Findings from two developmental clinics.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1063–1078. -
Saudino, K. J., Dale, P. S., Oliver, B., Petrill, S. A., Richardson, V., Rutter, M., & Plomin, R. (1998). The validity of parent-based assessment of the cognitive abilities of 2-year-olds.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 349–363. -
Snowling, M. J., Adams, J. W., Bishop, D. V. M., & Stothard, S. E. (2001). Educational attainments of school leavers with a preschool history of speech-language impairments.International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 173–183. - Statistics Netherlands. (2004). Standaard Onderwijsindeling 2003 [Categorization of the Dutch educational system 2003]. Voorburg/Heerlen, the Netherlands: Author.
-
Sterne, J. A., White, I. R., Carlin, J. B., Spratt, M., Royston, P., Kenward, M. G., & Carpenter, J. R. (2009). Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: Potential and pitfalls.British Medical Journal, 338, b2393. -
Thal, D., Tobias, S., & Morrison, D. (1991). Language and gesture in late talkers: A 1-year follow-up.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 604–612. -
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260. -
Westerlund, M., Berglund, E., & Eriksson, M. (2006). Can severely language delayed 3-year-olds be identified at 18 months? Evaluation of a screening version of the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 237–247. -
Wetherby, A., & Prizant, B. (2002). Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. -
Zink, I., & Lejaegere, M. (2002). Lijsten voor Communicatieve Ontwikkeling: Aanpassing en hernormering van de MacArthur CDIs van Fenson et al. [Communicative Development Inventories: Adaptation and norms of the Dutch version of the MacArthur CDIs by Fenson et al.]. Leuven, Belgium: Acco. -
Zink, I., & Lejaegere, M. (2003). N-CDIs: Korte vormen, aanpassing en hernormering van de MacArthur Short Form Vocabulary Checklists van Fenson et al. [N-CDIs: Short forms, adaptations and norms of the Dutch version of the short form of the MacArthur Vocabulary Checklists of Fenson et al.]. Leuven, Belgium: Acco. -
Zubrick, S. R., Taylor, C. L., Rice, M. L., & Slegers, D. W. (2007). Late language emergence at 24 months: An epidemiological study of prevalence, predictors, and covariates.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1562–1592.