No access
Research Article
April 2003

A Family Aggregation Study: The Influence of Family History and Other Risk Factors on Language Development

Publication: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
Volume 46, Number 2
Pages 261-272

Abstract

Substantial evidence continues to accrue for familial transmission of specific language impairment (SLI). The incidence in families with a history of SLI is estimated at approximately 20%–40%, whereas in the general population the estimated incidence is about 4%. Typical aggregation studies compare data on the speech and language status of parents and siblings of individuals with SLI (the probands) to similar data from family members of control individuals with no speech or language disorder history. In the present study, family aggregation of SLI was examined for a unique sample of children who were ascertained before 6 months of age and thus did not have SLI, but were born into a family with a positive history of SLI (FH+). No study to date has examined the pattern of affectance in families of children ascertained at such a young age. In addition, the ratio of boys to girls born into such families was investigated, as previous studies have suggested alterations in the expected gender ratios. Consistent with prior research, SLI was found to aggregate in families; the average affectance rate in FH+ families was 32%, with significantly more boys (41%) reported as having SLI than girls (16%). A comparison of FH+ and control families (FH–) on sociodemographic factors and medical history revealed differences in the overall rate of autoimmune diseases; FH+ families reported a significantly higher incidence (35%) compared to FH– families (9%). Finally, the 3- year language abilities of a subset of 32 children from FH+ families were compared with those of 60 children from FH– families. Children from FH+ families scored significantly lower on standardized measures of language and were more likely to fall below the 16th percentile (28%) than children from FH– families (7%). These results provide converging evidence that children from FH+ families are indeed at greater risk of developing language delay compared to children from control families.

Get full access to this article

View all available purchase options and get full access to this article.

References

Beitchman, J. H., Hood, J., & Inglis, A. (1992). Familial transmission of speech and language impairment: A preliminary investigation. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 37(3), 151–156.
Benasich, A. A. (2002). Impaired processing of brief, rapidly presented auditory cues in infants with a family history of autoimmune disorder. Developmental Neuropsychology, 22(1), 351–372.
Benasich, A. A., & Spitz, R. (1999). Insights from infants: Temporal processing abilities and genetics' contribution to language impairment. In K. Whitmore, H. Hary, & G. Willems (Eds.), A neurodevelopmental approach to specific learning disorders (pp. 191–209). London: Mac Keith Press.
Benasich, A. A., & Tallal P. (1998). Infant processing of auditory temporal information: Links to family history and later language outcome. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 24, 819.
Benasich, A. A., & Tallal, P. (2002). Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues: Links to family history and later language outcomes. Behavioral Brain Research, 136(1), 31–49.
Biederman, J., Milberger, S., Faraone, S. V., Lapey, K. A., Reed, E. D., & Seidman, L. J. (1995). No confirmation of Geshwind's hypothesis of associations between reading disability, immune disorders, and motor preference in ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 545–552.
Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Pre- and perinatal hazards and family background in children with specific language impairments: A study of twins. Brain and Language, 56, 1–26.
Bishop, D. V. M., & Edmundson, A. (1986). Is otitis media a major cause of specific developmental language disorders? British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 21, 321–338.
Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1995). Genetic basis of specific language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 37, 56–71.
Bryden, M. P., McManus, I. C., & Bulman-Flemming, M. B. (1994). Evaluating the empirical support for the Geschwind-Behan-Galaburda model of cerebral lateralization. Brain and Cognition, 26, 103–167.
Brzustowicz, L. (1996). Looking for language genes: Lessons from complex disorder studies. In M. L. Rice (Ed.), Toward a genetics of language (pp. 3–25). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dale, P. S., Simonoff, E., Bishop, D. V. M., Eley, T. C., Oliver, B., Price, T. S., et al. (1998). Genetic influence on language delay in two-year-old children. Nature Neuroscience, 1(4), 324–328.
Felsenfeld, S., McGue, M., & Broen, P. (1995). Familial aggregation of phonological disorders: Results from a 28-year follow-up. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 1091–1107.
Fenson, L. S., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., et al. (1993). Technical manual for the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. San Diego, CA: Singular Press.
Flannery, K. A., & Liederman, J. (1995). Is there really a syndrome involving co-occurrence of neurodevelopmental disorder, talent, non-right handedness and immune disorder among children? Cortex, 31, 503–515.
Flax, J., Realpe, T., Hirsch, L., Brzustowicz, L., Bartlett, C., & Tallal, P. (in press). Specific language impairment in families: Evidence for co-occurrence with reading impairment. Journal of Speech, Langauge, and Hearing Research.
Geschwind, N., & Behan, P. (1982). Left handedness: Association with immune disease, migraine, and developmental disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 79, 5097–5100.
Geschwind, N., & Behan, P. (1984). Laterality, hormones and immunity. In N. Geschwind & A. M. Galaburda (Eds.), Cerebral dominance: The biological foundations (pp. 211–224). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Gilger, J. W., Pennington, B. F., Green, P., Smith, S. A., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Dyslexia, immune disorders, and left-handedness: Twin and family studies of their relations. Neuropsychologia, 30, 209–227.
Gilger, J. W., Pennington, B. F., Harbeck, R. J., DeFries, J. C., Kotzin, B., Green, P., & Smith, S. (1998). A twin and family study of the association between immune system dysfunction and dyslexia using blood serum immunoassay and survey data. Brain and Cognition, 36, 310–333.
Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. B. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50.
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). The Four-Factor Index of Social Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University (Available from A. B. Hollingshead, Department of Sociology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520).
Hugdahl, K., Synnevag, B., & Satz, P. (1990). Immune and autoimmune disorders in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia, 28, 673–679.
Lahey, M., & Edwards, J. (1995). Specific language impairment: Preliminary investigation of factors associated with family history and with patterns of language performance. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 643–657.
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lewis, B. A. (1992). Pedigree analysis of children with phonology disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 586–597.
Lewis, B. A., & Thompson, L. A. (1992). A study of developmental speech and language disorders in twins. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1086–1094.
Neils, J., & Aram, D. M. (1986). Family history of children with developmental language disorders. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 655–658.
Pennington, B. F., Smith, S. D., Kimberling, W. J., Green, P. A., & Haith, M. M. (1987). Left-handedness and immune disorders in familial dyslexics. Archives of Neurology, 44, 634–639.
Plante, E., Shankman, K., & Clark, M. M. (1996). Classification of adults for family studies of developmental language disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 661–667.
Rice, M., Haney, K. R., & Wexler, K. (1998). Family histories of children with SLI who show extended optional infinitives. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 419–432.
Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Escobar, M. D. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, 998–1002.
Shriberg, L. D., Tomblin, B. J., & McSweeny, J. L. (1999). Prevalence of speech delay in 6-year-old children and comorbidity with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1461–1481.
Spitz, R. V., Tallal, P., Flax, J., & Benasich, A. A. (1997). Look who's talking: A prospective study of familial transmission of language impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 990–1001.
Stein, J. (2001). The neurobiology of reading difficulties. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 3–23). Timonium, MD: York Press.
Stromswold, K. (1998). Genetics of spoken language disorders. Human Biology, 70, 297–324.
Tallal, P., Hirsch, L. S., Realpe-Bonilla, T., Miller, S., Brzustowicz, L., Bartlett, C., & Flax, J. F. (2001). Family aggregation in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1172–1182.
Tallal, P., Ross, R., & Curtiss, S. (1989a). Familial aggregation in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Disorders, 54, 167–173.
Tallal, P., Ross, R., & Curtiss, S. (1989b). Unexpected sex-ratios in families of language/learning-impaired children. Neuropsychologia, 27, 987–998.
Tallal, P., Townsend, J., Curtiss, S., & Wulfeck, B. (1991). Phenotypic profiles of language-impaired children based on genetic/family history. Brain and Language, 41, 81–95.
Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). The Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale (4th ed.). Chicago: Riverside.
Tomblin, J. B. (1989). Familial concentration of developmental language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 287–295.
Tomblin, J. B. (1996). Genetic and environmental contributions to the risk for specific language impairment. In M. L. Rice (Ed.), Toward a genetics of language (pp. 191–211). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tomblin, J. B. (1997). Epidemiology of specific language impairment. In M. Gopnik (Ed.), The inheritance and innateness of grammars (pp. 91–109). New York: Oxford University Press.
Tomblin, J. B., & Buckwalter, P. R. (1994). Studies of genetics of specific language impairment. In R. V. Watkins & M. L. Rice (Eds.), Specific language impairments in children (Vol. 4, pp. 17–34). Baltimore: Brookes.
Tomblin, J. B., & Buckwalter, P. R. (1998). Heritability of poor language achievement among twins. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 188–199.
Tomblin, J. B., Hardy, J. C., & Hein, H. A. (1991). Predicting poor-communication status in preschool children using risk factors present at birth. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 1096–1105.
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Stollwerck, L. (1996). A grammatical specific language impairment in children: An autosomal dominant inheritance? Brain and Language, 52, 484–504
Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (1992). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals–Preschool: Examiners manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Wood, L. C., & Cooper, D. S. (1992). Autoimmune thyroid disease, left-handedness, and developmental dyslexia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 17, 95–99.
Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (1992). Preschool Language Scale–3. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
Volume 46Number 2April 2003
Pages: 261-272

History

  • Received: Feb 7, 2002
  • Accepted: Aug 13, 2002
  • Published in issue: Apr 1, 2003

Permissions

Request permissions for this article.

Keywords

  1. specific language impairment
  2. language
  3. aggregation
  4. learning impairments

Authors

Affiliations

Naseem Choudhury, PhD
Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Newark
April Ann Benasich
Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Newark

Notes

Contact author: Naseem Choudhury, PhD, Infancy Studies Laboratory, Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers University, 197 University Avenue, Newark, NJ 07102. E-mail: [email protected]

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Article Metrics
View all metrics



Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Citing Literature

  • Evidence-based preventive care guidelines in pediatric primary care, International Journal Of Community Medicine And Public Health, 10.18203/2394-6040.ijcmph20250075, (2025).
  • Deciphering the genetic basis of developmental language disorder in children without intellectual disability, autism or apraxia of speech, Molecular Autism, 10.1186/s13229-025-00642-8, 16, 1, (2025).
  • Late talking in young children in Saudi Arabia: Identifying key risk factors, First Language, 10.1177/01427237251313562, (2025).
  • Factors influencing language performance in boys and girls at age 2 in the French ELFE birth cohort, Brain Research, 10.1016/j.brainres.2024.149305, 1847, (149305), (2025).
  • The Impact of COVID-19 on ‘Spanish-Speaking’ Children’s Phonological Development, Education Sciences, 10.3390/educsci14080807, 14, 8, (807), (2024).
  • Language Delay in Children, Turkish Journal of Pediatric Disease, 10.12956/tchd.1475173, (1-6), (2024).
  • Familial aggregation of traffic risky behaviours among pedestrians: a cross-sectional study in northwestern Iran, Injury Prevention, 10.1136/ip-2023-045137, (ip-2023-045137), (2024).
  • Predictive Measures in Child Language Development: The Role of Familial History and Early Expressive Vocabulary, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 10.1044/2024_JSLHR-23-00815, 67, 10, (3714-3732), (2024).
  • Words, Language Development and Language Impairment: A Problem‐Based Introduction, 10.1002/9781394260287.ch4, (133-173), (2024).
  • What risk factors for Developmental Language Disorder can tell us about the neurobiological mechanisms of language development, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105398, 154, (105398), (2023).

View Options

Sign In Options

ASHA member? If so, log in with your ASHA website credentials for full access.

Member Login

View options

PDF

View PDF

Full Text

View Full Text

Figures

Tables

Media

Share

Share

Copy the content Link

Share