Purpose

The spacing effect in human memory research refers to situations in which people learn items better when they study items in spaced intervals rather than massed intervals. This investigation was conducted to compare the efficacy of meaning-oriented auditory training when administered with a spaced versus massed practice schedule.

Method

Forty-seven adult hearing aid users received 16 hr of auditory training. Participants in a spaced group (mean age = 64.6 years, SD = 14.7) trained twice per week, and participants in a massed group (mean age = 69.6 years, SD = 17.5) trained for 5 consecutive days each week. Participants completed speech perception tests before training, immediately following training, and then 3 months later. In line with transfer appropriate processing theory, tests assessed both trained tasks and an untrained task.

Results

Auditory training improved the speech recognition performance of participants in both groups. Benefits were maintained for 3 months. No effect of practice schedule was found on overall benefits achieved, on retention of benefits, nor on generalizability of benefits to nontrained tasks.

Conclusion

The lack of spacing effect in otherwise effective auditory training suggests that perceptual learning may be subject to different influences than are other types of learning, such as vocabulary learning. Hence, clinicians might have latitude in recommending training schedules to accommodate patients' schedules.

References

  • Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, L. E., Bahrick, A. S., & Bahrick, P. E. (1993). Maintenance of foreign language vocabulary and the spacing effect.Psychological Science, 4, 316–321.
  • Barcroft, J., Mauzé, E., Schroy, C., Tye-Murray, N., Sommers, M. & Spehar, B. (2011a). Improving the quality of auditory training by making tasks meaningful.Perspectives on Audiology, 7(1), 15–28.
  • Barcroft, J., Sommers, M. S., Tye-Murray, N., Mauzé, E., Schroy, C., & Spehar, B. (2011b). Tailoring auditory training to patient needs with single and multiple talkers: Transfer-appropriate gains on a four-choice discrimination test.International Journal of Audiology, 50, 802–808.
  • Barcroft, J., Spehar, B., Tye-Murray, N., & Sommers, M. S. (2016). Task- and talker-specific gains in auditory training.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 862–870.
  • Bello-Medina, P. C., Sánchez-Carrasco, L., González-Ornelas, N. R., Jeffrey, K. J., & Ramirez-Amaya, V. (2013). Differential effects of spaced vs. massed training in long-term object-identity and object-location recognition memory.Behavior Brain Research, 250, 102–113.
  • Burk, M. H., & Humes, L. E. (2008). Effects of long-term training on aided speech-recognition performance in noise in older adults.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 759–771.
  • Burk, M. H., Humes, L. E., Amos, N. E., & Strauser, L. E. (2006). Effect of training on word-recognition performance in noise for young normal-hearing and older hearing-impaired listeners.Ear and Hearing, 27, 263–278.
  • Carew, T. J., Pinsker, H. M., & Kandel, E. R. (1972). Long-term habituation of a defensive withdrawal reflex in aplysia.Science, 175, 451–454.
  • Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis.Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380.
  • Cornoldi, C., & Longoni, A. (1977). The MP-DP effect and the influence of distinct repetitions on recognition of random shapes.Italian Journal of Psychology, 4, 65–76.
  • Dempster, F. N. (1988). The spacing effect: A case study in the failure to apply the results of psychological research.American Psychologist, 43, 627–634.
  • Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Columbia University Press.
  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.
  • Fu, Q. J., Galvin, J., Wang, X., & Nogaki, G. (2004). Effects of auditory training on adult cochlear implant patients: A preliminary report.Cochlear Implants International, 5, 84–90.
  • Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2008). Exercising your brain: A review of human brain plasticity and training-induced learning.Psychology and Aging, 23, 692–701.
  • Henshaw, H., & Ferguson, M. A. (2013). Efficacy of individual computer-based auditory training for people with hearing loss: A systematic review of the evidence.PLoS One, 8, e62836.
  • Hintzman, D. L. (1974). Theoretical implications of the spacing effect.In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola symposium (pp. 77–97). Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.
  • Humes, L. E., Burk, M. H., Strauser, L. E., & Kinney, D. L. (2009). Development and efficacy of a frequent-word auditory training protocol for older adults with impaired hearing.Ear & Hearing, 30(5), 613–627. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181b00d90
  • Kornell, N., Castel, A. D., Eich, T. S., & Bjork, R. A. (2010). Spacing as the friend of both memory and indication in younger and older adults.Psychology and Aging, 25, 498–503.
  • Lee, T. D., & Genovese, E. D. (1988). Distribution of practice in motor skill acquisition: Learning and performance effects reconsidered.Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 59, 277–287.
  • Levitt, H. (1970). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 33, 467–476.
  • McDaniel, M. A., Fadler, C. L., & Pashler, H. (2013). Effects of spaced versus massed training in function learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1417–1432.
  • Melton, A. W. (1967). Repetition and retrieval from memory.Science, 158, 532–532.
  • Molloy, K., Moore, D. R., Sohoglu, E., & Amitay, S. (2012). Less is more: Latent learning is maximized by shorter training sessions in auditory perceptual learning.PLoS One, 7, e36929.
  • Moss, V. D. (1996). The efficacy of massed versus distributed practice as a function of desired learning outcomes and grade level of the student.Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 5204.
  • Oba, S. I., Fu, Q. J., & Galvin, J. J., III. (2011). Digit training in noise can improve cochlear implant users' speech understanding in noise.Ear and Hearing, 32, 573–581.
  • Russo, R., Parkin, A., Taylor, S., & Wilks, J. (1998). Revising current two-process accounts of spacing effects in memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 819–829.
  • Shebilske, W. L., Goettl, B. P., Corrington, K., & Day, E. A. (1999). Interlesson spacing and task-related processing during complex skill acquisition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5, 413–437.
  • Simone, P. M., Bell, M. C., & Cepeda, N. J. (2012). Diminished but not forgotten: Effects of aging on magnitude of spacing effect benefits.Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 68, 674–680. https://doi.org/10.1093/geron/gbs096
  • Sisti, H. M., Glass, A. L., & Shors, T. J. (2007). Neurogenesis and the spacing effect: Learning over time enhances memory and the survival of new neurons.Learning and Memory, 14, 368–375.
  • Slater, A. (1998). Perceptual development: Visual, auditory, and speech perception in infancy. Hove, England: Psychology Press.
  • Stacey, P. C., Raine, C. H., O'Donoghue, G. M., Tapper, L., Twomey, T., & Summerfield, A. Q. (2010). Effectiveness of computer-based auditory training for adult users of cochlear implants.International Journal of Audiology, 49, 347–356.
  • Stecker, G. C., Bowman, G. A., Yund, E. W., Herron, T. J., Roup, C. M., & Woods, D. L. (2006). Perceptual training improves syllable identification in new and experienced hearing aid users.Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 43, 537–552.
  • Sweetow, R., & Palmer, C. V. (2005). Efficacy of individual auditory training in adults: A systematic review of the evidence.Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 16, 494–504.
  • Sweetow, R. W., & Henderson-Sabes, J. (2006). The need for and development of an adaptive listening and communication enhancement (LACE™) program.Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 17, 538–558.
  • Tye-Murray, N. (2015). Foundations of aural rehabilitation: Children, adults, and their family members. Stamford, CT: Cengage Learning.
  • Tye-Murray, N., Sommers, M., & Barcroft, J. (2011). I hear what you mean: The state of the science in auditory training.ENT and Audiology News, 20(4), 84–86.
  • Tye-Murray, N., Sommers, M., Spehar, B., Myerson, J., Hale, S., & Rose, N. (2008). Auditory-visual discourse comprehension by older and young adults in favorable and unfavorable conditions.International Journal of Audiology, 47, S31–S37.
  • Tye-Murray, N., Spehar, B., Sommers, M. S., & Barcroft, J. (2016). Auditory training with frequent communication partners.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 871–875.
  • Vlach, H. A. (2014). The spacing effect in children's generalization of knowledge: Allowing children time to forget promotes their ability to learn.Child Development Perspectives, 8(3), 163–168.
  • Wang, Z., Zhou, R., & Shah, P. (2014). Spaced cognitive training promotes training transfer.Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 217. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00217
  • Zhang, T., Dorman, M. F., Fu, Q. J., & Spahr, A. J. (2012). Auditory training in patients with unilateral cochlear implant and contralateral acoustic stimulation.Ear and Hearing, 33, e70–e79.

Additional Resources