No access
Research Note
17 May 2018

Does Implicit Voice Learning Improve Spoken Language Processing? Implications for Clinical Practice

Publication: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
Volume 61, Number 5
Pages 1251-1260

Abstract

Purpose

In typical interactions with other speakers, including a clinical environment, listeners become familiar with voices through implicit learning. Previous studies have found evidence for a Familiar Talker Advantage (better speech perception and spoken language processing for familiar voices) following explicit voice learning. The current study examined whether a Familiar Talker Advantage would result from implicit voice learning.

Method

Thirty-three adults and 16 second graders were familiarized with 1 of 2 talkers' voices over 2 days through live interactions as 1 of 2 experimenters administered standardized tests and interacted with the listeners. To assess whether this implicit voice learning would generate a Familiar Talker Advantage, listeners completed a baseline sentence recognition task and a post-learning sentence recognition task with both the familiar talker and the unfamiliar talker.

Results

No significant effect of voice familiarity was found for either the children or the adults following implicit voice learning. Effect size estimates suggest that familiarity with the voice may benefit some listeners, despite the lack of an overall effect of familiarity.

Discussion

We discuss possible clinical implications of this finding and directions for future research.

Get full access to this article

View all available purchase options and get full access to this article.

References

Allen, J. S., & Miller, J. L. (2004). Listener sensitivity to individual talker differences in voice-onset-time. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 115(6), 3171–3183.
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1997). Omnibus survey results. Rockville, MD: Author.
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48.
Benkı́, J. R. (2003). Quantitative evaluation of lexical status, word frequency, and neighborhood density as context effects in spoken word recognition. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113(3), 1689–1705.
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2016). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.0.23) [Computer program]. Retrieved from: http://www.praat.org
Ebert, K. D. (2017). Measuring clinician–client relationships in speech-language treatment for school-age children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 26(1), 146–152.
Eisner, F., & McQueen, J. M. (2005). The specificity of perceptual learning in speech processing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 67(2), 224–238.
Felty, R. A. (2007). Context effects in spoken word recognition of English and German by native and non-native listeners. East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University Press.
Goldinger, S. D. (1996). Words and voices: Episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(5), 1166–1183.
Hoffman, L. (2014). Prologue: Improving clinical practice from the inside out. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 45(2), 89–91.
Huyck, J. J., Smith, R. H., Hawkins, S., & Johnsrude, I. S. (2017). Generalization of perceptual learning of degraded speech across talkers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(11), 3334–3341.
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, 20 U.S.C § 1400 (2004).
Ireland, M., & Conrad, B. J. (2016). Evaluation and eligibility for speech-language services in schools. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 1(16), 78–90.
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M., Lee, K., Xie, X., Huang, H., Ye, H. H., … Wang, Z. (2003). Effects of experience on fetal voice recognition. Psychological Science, 14(3), 220–224.
Kraljic, T., & Samuel, A. G. (2007). Perceptual adjustments to multiple speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(1), 1–15.
Lenth, R. V. (2016). Least-squares means: The R package lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software, 69(1), 1–33.
Levi, S. V. (2015). Talker familiarity and spoken word recognition in school-age children. Journal of Child Language, 42(4), 843–872.
Levi, S. V., Winters, S. J., & Pisoni, D. B. (2011). Effects of cross-language voice training on speech perception: Whose familiar voices are more intelligible? The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(6), 4053–4062.
McQueen, J. M., Cutler, A., & Norris, D. (2006). Phonological abstraction in the mental lexicon. Cognitive Science, 30(6), 1113–1126.
Nelson, P., Kohnert, K., Sabur, S., & Shaw, D. (2005). Classroom noise and children learning through a second language: Double jeopardy? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36(3), 219–229.
New York City Department of Education. (2009). Standard operating procedures manual: The referral, evaluation, and placement of school-age students with disabilities. New York, NY: Author.
New York City Early Intervention System. (2014). Policy and Procedure Manual. New York, NY: Author.
Norris, D., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2003). Perceptual learning in speech. Cognitive Psychology, 47(2), 204–238.
Nygaard, L. C., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Talker-specific learning in speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 60(3), 355–376.
Nygaard, L. C., Sommers, M. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (1994). Speech perception as a talker-contingent process. Psychological Science, 5(1), 42–46.
Peña, E. D., & Quinn, R. (1997). Task familiarity: Effects on the test performance of Puerto Rican and African American children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 28(4), 323–332.
R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. ISBN 3-900051-07-0. Retrieved from http://www.r-project.org/
Samuel, A. G., & Kraljic, T. (2009). Perceptual learning for speech. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(6), 1207–1218.
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccoloto, A. (2007). E-Prime 2.0 Professional. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychological Software Tools, Inc.
Schroeder, M. (1968). Reference signal for signal quality studies. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 44(6), 1735–1736.
Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (2004). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool–Second Edition. Bloomington, MN: Pearson.
Smith, G. W., & Riccomini, P. J. (2013). The effect of a noise reducing test accommodation on elementary students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 28(2), 89–95.
Souza, P., Gehani, N., Wright, R., & McCloy, D. (2013). The advantage of knowing the talker. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 24(8), 689–700.
Stelmachowicz, P. G., Hoover, B. M., Lewis, D. E., Kortekaas, R. W., & Pittman, A. L. (2000). The relation between stimulus context, speech audibility, and perception for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(4), 902–914.
Theodore, R. M., & Miller, J. L. (2010). Characteristics of listener sensitivity to talker-specific phonetic detail a. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128(4), 2090–2099.
Theodore, R. M., Miller, J. L., & DeSteno, D. (2009). Individual talker differences in voice-onset-time: Contextual influences a. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(6), 3974–3982.
Trude, A. M., & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2012). Talker-specific perceptual adaptation during online speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(7–8), 979–1001.
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373.
Yonan, C. A., & Sommers, M. S. (2000). The effects of talker familiarity on spoken word identification in younger and older listeners. Psychology and Aging, 15(1), 88–99.
Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002). Preschool Language Scale–Fourth Edition (PLS-4). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
Volume 61Number 517 May 2018
Pages: 1251-1260
PubMed: 29800358

History

  • Received: Aug 10, 2017
  • Revised: Dec 2, 2017
  • Accepted: Jan 19, 2018
  • Published in issue: May 17, 2018

Permissions

Request permissions for this article.

Authors

Affiliations

Julie Case
Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, New York University, New York
Scott Seyfarth
Department of Linguistics and Office of Academic Affairs, Ohio State University, Columbus
Susannah V. Levi
Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, New York University, New York

Notes

Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time of publication.
Correspondence to Julie Case: [email protected]
Editor-in-Chief: Sean Redmond
Editor: Lizbeth Finestack

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Article Metrics
View all metrics



Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Citing Literature

  • Relating pronunciation distance metrics to intelligibility across English accents, Journal of Phonetics, 10.1016/j.wocn.2024.101357, 107, (101357), (2024).
  • Intelligibility as a measure of speech perception: Current approaches, challenges, and recommendations, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 10.1121/10.0016806, 153, 1, (68-76), (2023).
  • Can Playing a Game Improve Children's Speech Recognition? A Preliminary Study of Implicit Talker Familiarity Effects, American Journal of Audiology, 10.1044/2023_AJA-23-00156, 33, 1, (183-198), (2023).
  • The Effect of Talker Familiarity on Sentence Recognition Accuracy in Complex Noise, Experimental Psychology, 10.1027/1618-3169/a000509, 68, 1, (49-55), (2021).
  • Who We Are, The Handbook of Speech Perception, 10.1002/9781119184096.ch14, (365-397), (2021).
  • Short-term implicit voice-learning leads to a Familiar Talker Advantage: The role of encoding specificity, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 10.1121/1.5081469, 144, 6, (EL497-EL502), (2018).

View Options

Sign In Options

ASHA member? If so, log in with your ASHA website credentials for full access.

Member Login

View options

PDF

View PDF

Full Text

View Full Text

Figures

Tables

Media

Share

Share

Copy the content Link

Share