Abstract
Purpose
Retrieval practice has been found to be a powerful strategy to enhance long-term retention of new information; however, the utility of retrieval practice when teaching young children new words is largely unknown, and even less is known for young children with language impairments. The current study examined the effect of 2 different retrieval schedules on word learning at both the behavioral and neural levels.
Method
Participants included 16 typically developing children (MTD = 61.58 months) and 16 children with developmental language disorder (MDLD = 59.60 months). Children participated in novel word learning sessions in which the spacing of retrieval practice was manipulated: Some words were retrieved only after other words had been presented (i.e., repeated retrieval that required contextual reinstatement [RRCR]); others were taught using an immediate retrieval schedule. In Experiment 1, children's recall of the novel word labels and their meanings was tested after a 5-min delay and a 1-week delay. In Experiment 2, event-related brain potentials were obtained from a match–mismatch task utilizing the novel word stimuli.
Results
Experiment 1 findings revealed that children were able to label referents and to retain the novel words more successfully if the words were taught in the RRCR learning condition. Experiment 2 findings revealed that mismatching picture–word pairings elicited a robust N400 event-related brain potential only for words that were taught in the RRCR condition. In addition, children were more accurate in identifying picture–word matches and mismatches for words taught in the RRCR condition, relative to the immediate retrieval condition.
Conclusions
Retrieval practice that requires contextual reinstatement through spacing results in enhanced word learning and long-term retention of words. Both typically developing children and children with developmental language disorder benefit from this type of retrieval procedure.
Supplemental Material
References
-
Alt, M., Meyers, C., Oglivie, T., Nicholas, K., & Arizmendi, G. (2014). Cross-situational statistically based word learning intervention for late-talking toddlers.Journal of Communication Disorders, 52, 207–220. -
Alt, M., & Plante, E. (2006). Factors that influence lexical and semantic fast mapping language impairment.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 941–955. - American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1997). Guidelines for audiologic screening. Retrieved from http://www.asha.org/policy
-
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4.Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. -
Batterink, L., & Neville, H. (2011). Implicit and explicit mechanisms of word learning in a narrative context: An event-related potential study.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3181–3196. -
Besson, M., Kutas, M., & Petten, C. V. (1992). An event-related potential (ERP) analysis of semantic congruity and repetition effects in sentences.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 132–149. -
Bjork, R. A. (1988). Retrieval practice and the maintenance of knowledge.Practical Aspects of Memory: Current Research and Issues, Vol. 1: Memory in Everyday Life, 1, 396–401. -
Boersma, M., & Weenink, D. (2006). PRAAT: Doing phonetics by computer. the Netherlands: University of Amsterdam. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org -
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 1142–1157. -
Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Rohrer, D., & Wixted, J. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative syntesis.Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380. -
Dawson, J., Stout, C., Eyer, J. A., Tattersall, P., Fonkalsrud, J., & Croley, K. (2005). Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test–Preschool 2. DeKalb, IL: Janelle. -
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Fourth Edition (PPVT-4). Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson. -
Edwards, J., Beckman, M. E., & Munson, B. (2004). Vocabulary size and phonotactic production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 421–436. -
Ellis, E. M., Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Evans, J. L. (2015). Novel word learning: An eye-tracking study. Are 18-month-old late talkers really different from their typical peers.Journal of Communication Disorders, 58, 143–157. -
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (2001). Meaning and modality: Influences of context, semantic memory organization, and perceptual predictability on picture processing.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 202–224. -
Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Lexical priming and semantic integration reflected in the event-related potential of 14-month-olds.Neuroreport, 16, 653–656. -
Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., Nolan, D., & Singleton, J. (2007). Expanding retrieval practice: An effective aid to preschool children's learning.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 991–1004. -
Gertner, B. L., Rice, M. L., & Hadley, P. A. (1994). Influence of communicative competence on peer preferences in a preschool classroom.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 913–923. -
Goossens, N. A. M. C., Camp, G., Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., Tabbers, H. K., & Zwaan, R. A. (2014). The benefit of retrieval practice over elaborative restudy in primary school vocabulary learning.Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3, 177–182. -
Gray, S. (2004). Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: Predictors and poor learners.Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 47, 1117–1132. -
Greenslade, K. J., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (2009). The diagnostic accuracy and construct validity of the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test–Preschool 2.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40, 150–160. -
Grier, J. B. (1971). Nonparametric indexes for sensitivity and bias: Computing formulas.Psychological Bulletin, 75, 424–429. -
Haebig, E., Leonard, L. B., Usler, E., Deevy, P., & Weber, C. (2018). An initial investigation of the neural correlates of word processing in preschoolers with specific language impairment.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 729–739. -
Haebig, E., Saffran, J. R., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2017). Statistical word learning in children with autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment.The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 1251–1263. -
Hahne, A., Eckstein, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Brain signatures of syntactic and semantic processes during children's language development.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1302–1318. -
Hall, W. S., Nagy, W. E., & Linn, R. L. (1984). Spoken words, effects of situation and social group on oral word usage and frequency. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. -
Heisler, L., Goffman, L., & Younger, B. (2010). Lexical and articulatory interactions in children's language production.Developmental Science, 13, 722–730. -
Holcomb, P. J., Coffey, S. A., & Neville, H. J. (1992). Visual and auditory sentence processing: A developmental analysis using event-related brain potentials.Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 203–241. -
Juottonen, K., Revonsuo, A., & Lang, H. (1996). Dissimilar age influences on two ERP waveforms (LPC and N400) reflecting semantic context effect.Cognitive Brain Research, 4, 99–107. -
Jurcak, V., Tsuzuki, D., & Dan, I. (2007). 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: Their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems.NeuroImage, 34, 1600–1611. -
Justice, L. M., Meier, J., & Walpole, S. (2005). Learning new words from storybooks: An efficacy study with at-risk kindergartners.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 17–32. -
Kail, R. V., Hale, C. A., Leonard, L. B., & Nippold, M. A. (1984). Lexical storage and retrieval in language-impaired children.Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 37–49. -
Kail, R. V., & Leonard, L. B. (1986). Word-finding abilities in language-impaired children.ASHA Monographs, 25, 1–39. -
Kan, P. F., & Windsor, J. (2010). Word learning in children with primary language impairment: A meta-analysis.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 739–757. -
Karpicke, J. D., & Bauernschmidt, A. (2011). Spaced retrieval: Absolute spacing enhances learning regardless of relative spacing.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1250–1257. -
Karpicke, J. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping.Science, 331, 772–775. -
Karpicke, J. D., Blunt, J. R., & Smith, M. A. (2016). Retrieval-based learning: Positive effects of retrieval practice in elementary school children.Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–9. -
Karpicke, J. D., Lehman, M., & Aue, W. R. (2014). Retrieval-based learning: An episodic context account.InB. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 60). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Presshttps://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800283-4.00007-1 -
Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). The critical importance of retrieval for learning.Science, 319, 966–968. -
Kaufmann, A., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. Circle Pines, MN: AGS. -
Kornilov, S. A., Magnuson, J. S., Rakhlin, N., Landi, N., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2015). Lexical processing deficits in children with developmental language disorder: An event-related potentials study.Development and Psychopathology, 27, 459–476. -
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP).Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647. -
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading sensless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity.Science, 207, 203–205. -
Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning.InM. M. Gruneberg & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 625–632). London, England: Academic Press. -
Lee, L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. -
Lehman, M., Smith, M. A., & Karpicke, J. D. (2014). Toward an episodic context account of retrieval-based learning: Dissociating retrieval practice and elaboration.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 40, 1787–1794. -
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. -
Leonard, L. B., Karpicke, J., Weber, C., Deevy, P., Christ, S., Haebig, E., Souto, S., Keuser, J., & Krok, W. (2019). Retrieval-based word learning in typically developing children and children with developmental language disorder I: The benefits of repeated retrieval.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0070 -
Leonard, L. B., Nippold, M. A., Kail, R. V., & Hale, C. A. (1983). Picture naming in language-impaired children.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 609–615. -
Linebarger, M. C., Schwartz, M. F., & Saffran, E. M. (1983). Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics.Cognition, 13, 361–392. -
Lopez-Calderon, J., & Luck, S. J. (2010). ERPLAB toolbox (1.1.0). Retrieved from http://erpinfo.org/erplab -
Lucas, R., & Norbury, C. F. (2015). Making inferences from text: It's vocabulary that matters.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 1224–1232. -
Luck, S. J. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential technique (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: MIT Press. -
McGregor, K. K. (2014). What a difference a day makes: Change in memory for newly learned word forms over twenty-four hours.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, 1842–1850. -
McGregor, K. K., Gordon, K., Eden, N., Arbisi-Kelm, T., & Oleson, J. (2017). Encoding deficits impede word learning and memory in adults with developmental language disorders.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 2891–2905. -
McGregor, K. K., Oleson, J., Bahnsen, A., & Duff, D. (2013). Children with developmental language impairment have vocabulary deficits characterized by limited breadth and depth.International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48, 307–319. -
McMurray, B., Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. K. (2012). Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning.Psychological Review, 119, 831–877. -
Mills, D. L., Coffey-Corina, S. A., & Neville, H. J. (1993). Language acquisition and cerebral specialization in 20-month-old infants.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 317–334. -
Neville, H., Coffey, S. A., Holcomb, P. J., & Tallal, P. (1993). The neurobiology of sensory and language processing in language-impaired children.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 235–253. -
Oetting, J. B., Rice, M. L., & Swank, L. K. (1995). Quick incidental learning (QUIL) of words by school age children with and without SLI.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 434–445. -
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory.Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. -
Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What's meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension.Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 554–566. -
Pijnacker, J., Davids, N., Van Weerdenburg, M., Verhoeven, L., Knoors, H., & Van Alphen, P. (2017). Semantic processing of sentences in preschoolers with specific language impairment: Evidence from the N400 effect.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 627–639. -
Quinn, J. M., Wagner, R. K., Petscher, Y., & Lopez, D. (2015). Developmental relations between vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension: A latent change score modeling study.Child Development, 86, 159–175. -
Renoult, L., Brodeur, M. B., & Debruille, J. B. (2010). Semantic processing of highly repeated concepts presented in single-word trials: Electrophysiological and behavioral correlates.Biological Psychology, 84, 206–220. -
Renoult, L., & Debruille, J. B. (2009). N400-like potentials and reaction times index semantic relations between highly repeated individual words.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 905–922. -
Rice, M. L., & Hoffman, L. (2015). Predicting vocabulary growth in children with and without specific language impairment: A longitudinal study from 2;6 to 21 years of age.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 345–359. -
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Redmond, S. M. (1999). Grammaticality judments of extended optional infinitive grammar: Evidence from english-speaking children with specific langauge impairment.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 943–961. -
Roark, B., & Demuth, K. (2000). Prosodic constraints and the learner's environment: A corpus study.InS. C. Howell, S. A. Fish, & T. Keith-Lucas (Eds.), BUCLD 24: Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 597–608). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. -
Sabisch, B., Hahne, A., Glass, E., von Suchodoletz, W., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Lexical-semantic processes in children with specific language impairment.NeuroReport, 17, 1511–1514. -
Schopler, E., Van Bourgondien, M., Wellman, G., & Love, S. (2010). Childhood Autism Rating Scale–Second Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. -
Silva-Pereyra, J., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2005). An event-related brain potential study of sentence comprehension in preschoolers: Semantic and morphosyntactic processing.Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 247–258. -
Storkel, H. L., & Hoover, J. R. (2010). An online calculator to compute phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on the basis of child corpora of spoken American English.Behavior Research Methods, 42, 497–506. -
Storkel, H. L., Komesidou, R., Fleming, K. K., & Romine, R. S. (2017). Interactive book reading to accelerate word learning by kindergarten children with specific language impairment: Identifying adequate progress and successful learning patterns.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 48, 108–124. -
Storkel, H. L., Voelmle, K., Fierro, V., Flake, K., Fleming, K. K., & Romine, R. S. (2017). Interactive book reading to accelerate word learning by kindergarten children with specific language impairment: Identifying an adequate intensity and variation in treatment response.Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 48, 16–30. -
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O, Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260. -
Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis.Cortex, 41, 399–433. -
Usler, E., & Weber-Fox, C. (2015). Neurodevelopment for syntactic processing distinguishes childhood stuttering recovery versus persistence.Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 7, 1–21.