Purpose

We compared the narrative production in Mandarin-speaking children at risk (AR) for developmental language disorder (DLD) and typically developing (TD) controls to address two goals: (a) further our understanding of the Mandarin DLD phenotype and (b) examine the role of elicitation method in differentiating AR from TD.

Method

Twenty-one AR children and 21 age- and nonverbal IQ–matched peers produced two stories from the Multilingual Assessment Instrument of Narrative, first following an adult model (i.e., story-retell) and then without a model (i.e., story-tell). Group and task effects were analyzed on macrostructure and microstructure measures.

Results

For general macrostructure score and sentence complexity, children in the AR group performed more poorly than TD children on the more challenging story-tell task and showed decreased scores from retell to tell tasks. In addition, children in the AR group showed poorer performance on number of different words. Productivity and grammaticality measures did not show group differences.

Discussion

Consistent with previous findings, grammaticality and productivity were relatively preserved but story macrostructure, lexical diversity, and sentence complexity were vulnerable in Mandarin-speaking children with or AR for DLD. Having an adult model benefited both groups in sentence complexity and story macrostructure and potentially helped maintain the performance in TD children as they engaged in the more challenging story-telling task.

References

References

  • Balthazar, C. H., & Scott, C. M. (2017). Complex sentence intervention.In R. J. McCauley, M. E. Fey, & R. B. Gillam (Eds.), Treatment of language disorders in children (2nd ed., pp. 349–387). Brookes.
  • Chao, Y. R. (1968). A grammar of spoken Chinese. University of California Press.
  • Cheung, H. (2009). Grammatical characteristics of Mandarin speaking children with specific language impairment.In S. Law, B. Weekes, & A. M.-Y. Wong (Eds.), Language disorders in speakers of Chinese (pp. 33–52). Multilingual Matters.
  • Hunt, K. W. (1965). Grammatical structures written at three grade levels. National Council of Teachers of English.
  • Klee, T., Stokes, S. F., Wong, A. M.-Y., Fletcher, P., & Gavin, W. J. (2004). Utterance length and lexical diversity in Cantonese-speaking children with and without specific language impairment.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1396–1410. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/104)
  • Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1981). Mandarin Chinese: A function reference grammar. University of California Press.
  • Loban, W. (1976). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve (Vol. 18). National Council of Teachers.
  • Sah, W.-H. (2013). Global and local connections in Mandarin-speaking children's narratives.In A. McCabe & C.-J. Chang (Eds.), Chinese language narration: Culture, cognition, and emotion (pp. 85–114). John Benjamins.
  • SALT Software, LLC. (2016). C-unit segmentation rules. SALT Software, LLC. http://www.saltsoftware.com/coursefiles/shared/Cunits.pdf
  • To, C. K.-S., Stokes, S. F., Cheung, H.-T., & T'sou, B. (2010). Narrative assessment for Cantonese-speaking children.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(3), 648–669. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0039)
  • Torng, P.-C., & Sah, W.-H. (2019). Narrative abilities of Mandarin-speaking children with and without specific language impairment: Macrostructure and microstructure.Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2019.1655097
  • Tsai, W., & Chang, C. (2008). “But I first…and then he kept picking” Narrative skill in Mandarin-speaking children with language impairment.Narrative Inquiry, 18(2), 349–377. https://doi.org/10.1075/ni.18.2.09tsa
  • Zhang, F. (2013). A study of narrative development young Chinese children with specific language impairment aged four to six years.In A. McCabe & C.-J. Chang (Eds.), Chinese Language narration: Culture, cognition, and emotion (pp. 143–179). John Benjamins.

Additional Resources