No access
Editor's Award
Research Article
22 January 2020

Parental Language Input to Children With Hearing Loss: Does It Matter in the End?

Publication: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
Volume 63, Number 1
Pages 234-258

Abstract

Purpose

Parental language input (PLI) has reliably been found to influence child language development for children at risk of language delay, but previous work has generally restricted observations to the preschool years. The current study examined whether PLI during the early years explains variability in the spoken language abilities of children with hearing loss at those young ages, as well as later in childhood.

Participants

One hundred children participated: 34 with normal hearing, 24 with moderate losses who used hearing aids (HAs), and 42 with severe-to-profound losses who used cochlear implants (CIs). Mean socioeconomic status was middle class for all groups. Children with CIs generally received them early.

Method

Samples of parent–child interactions were analyzed to characterize PLI during the preschool years. Child language abilities (CLAs) were assessed at 48 months and 10 years of age.

Results

No differences were observed across groups in how parents interacted with their children. Nonetheless, strong differences across groups were observed in the effects of PLI on CLAs at 48 months of age: Children with normal hearing were largely resilient to their parents' language styles. Children with HAs were most influenced by the amount of PLI. Children with CIs were most influenced by PLI that evoked child language and modeled more complex versions. When potential influences of preschool PLI on CLAs at 10 years of age were examined, those effects at preschool were replicated. When mediation analyses were performed, however, it was found that the influences of preschool PLI on CLAs at 10 years of age were partially mediated by CLAs at preschool.

Conclusion

PLI is critical to the long-term spoken language abilities of children with hearing loss, but the style of input that is most effective varies depending on the severity of risk for delay.

Get full access to this article

View all available purchase options and get full access to this article.

References

Bowey, J. A., & Hirakis, E. (2006). Testing the protracted lexical restructuring hypothesis: The effects of position and acoustic–phonetic clarity on sensitivity to mispronunciations in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 1–17.
de Boysson-Bardies, B., Sagart, L., Halle, P., & Durand, C. (1986). Acoustic investigations of cross-linguistic variability in babbling. In B. Lindblom & R. Zetterström (Eds.), Wenner-Gren Center International Symposium Series. Vol:44. Precursors of early speech (pp. 113–126). New York, NY: Stockton Press.
Brownell, R. (2000). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition (EOWPVT-3). Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.
Caldwell, A., & Nittrouer, S. (2013). Speech perception in noise by children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 13–30.
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language (CASL). Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessments.
Charles-Luce, J., & Luce, P. A. (1990). Similarity neighbourhoods of words in young children's lexicons. Journal of Child Language, 17, 205–215.
Davidson, K., Lillo-Martin, D., & Chen Pichler, D. (2014). Spoken English language development among native signing children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19, 238–250.
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories–Second Edition. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51, 419–439.
Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and written story composition skills of children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 1301–1318.
Friel-Patti, S., & Finitzo, T. (1990). Language learning in a prospective study of otitis media with effusion in the first two years of life. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 188–194.
Gravel, J. S., & Wallace, I. F. (1992). Listening and language at 4 years of age: Effects of early otitis media. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 588–595.
Hamdan, F. F., Daoud, H., Rochefort, D., Piton, A., Gauthier, J., Langlois, M., … Michaud, J. L. (2010). De novo mutations in FOXP1 in cases with intellectual disability, autism, and language impairment. American Journal of Human Genetics, 87, 671–678.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Hess, R. D., & Shipman, V. C. (1965). Early experience and the socialization of cognitive modes in children. Child Development, 36, 869–886.
Hirsh, I. J., Davis, H., Silverman, S. R., Reynolds, E. G., Eldert, E., & Benson, R. W. (1952). Development of materials for speech audiometry. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 17, 321–337.
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11, F31–F39.
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365.
Kent, R. N., & Foster, S. L. (1977). Direct observational procedures: Methodological issues in naturalistic settings. In A. R. Ciminero, K. S. Calhoun, & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Handbook of behavioral assessment (pp. 279–328). Toronto, Canada: Wiley.
Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9, F13–F21.
Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413, 519–523.
Laosa, L. M. (1982). Families as facilitators of children's intellectual development at 3 years of age: A causal analysis. In L. M. Laosa & I. E. Sigel (Eds.), Families as learning environments for children (pp. 1–45). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Leslie, L., & Caldwell, J. S. (2006). Qualitative Reading Inventory (Vol. 4). New York, NY: Pearson.
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F. W., & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201–212.
MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593–614.
Mahr, T., & Edwards, J. (2018). Using language input and lexical processing to predict vocabulary size. Developmental Science, 21, e12685.
Menn, L. (1978). Phonological units in beginning speech. In A. Bell & J. B. Hooper (Eds.), Syllables and segments (pp. 157–172). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland Publishing Company.
Menn, L. (1983). Development of articulatory, phonetic and phonological capabilities. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production: Development, writing and other language processes (pp. 3–50). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Menyuk, P., Menn, L., & Silber, R. (1979). Early strategies for the perception and production of words and sounds. In P. Fletcher & M. Garman (Eds.), Language acquisition (pp. 49–70). Massachusetts: Cambridge University Press.
Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Miller, J., & Iglesias, A. (2006). Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT): Version 9. Madison: University of Wisconsin–Madison, Waisman Center, Language Analysis Laboratory.
Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: AGS.
Napoli, D. J., Mellon, N. K., Niparko, J. K., Rathmann, C., Mathur, G., Humphries, T., … Lantos, J. D. (2015). Should all deaf children learn sign language? Pediatrics, 136, 170–176.
Nittrouer, S. (1996). The relation between speech perception and phonemic awareness: Evidence from low-SES children and children with chronic OM. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1059–1070.
Nittrouer, S. (2002). From ear to cortex: A perspective on what clinicians need to understand about speech perception and language processing. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 237–251.
Nittrouer, S. (2010). Early development of children with hearing loss. San Diego, CA: Plural.
Nittrouer, S., & Burton, L. T. (2005). The role of early language experience in the development of speech perception and phono-logical processing abilities: Evidence from 5-year-olds with histories of otitis media with effusion and low socioeconomic status. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38, 29–63.
Nittrouer, S., Caldwell, A., & Holloman, C. (2012). Measuring what matters: Effectively predicting language and literacy in children with cochlear implants. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 76, 1148–1158.
Nittrouer, S., Caldwell-Tarr, A., Tarr, E., Lowenstein, J. H., Rice, C., & Moberly, A. C. (2013). Improving speech-in-noise recognition for children with hearing loss: Potential effects of language abilities, binaural summation, and head shadow. International Journal of Audiology, 52, 513–525.
Nittrouer, S., & Lowenstein, J. H. (2015). Weighting of acoustic cues to a manner distinction by children with and without hearing loss. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 1077–1092.
Nittrouer, S., Muir, M., Tietgens, K., Moberly, A. C., & Lowenstein, J. H. (2018). Development of phonological, lexical, and syntactic abilities in children with cochlear implants across the elementary grades. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 2561–2577.
Nittrouer, S., Sansom, E., Low, K., Rice, C., & Caldwell-Tarr, A. (2014). Language structures used by kindergartners with cochlear implants: Relationship to phonological awareness, lexical knowledge and hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 35, 506–518.
Norman-Jackson, J. (1982). Family interactions, language development, and primary reading achievement of Black children in families of low income. Child Development, 53, 349–358.
Nudel, R., & Newbury, D. F. (2013). FOXP2. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4, 547–560.
Onnis, L., Truzzi, A., & Ma, X. (2018). Language development and disorders: Possible genes and environment interactions. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 82, 132–146.
Quittner, A. L., Cruz, I., Barker, D. H., Tobey, E., Eisenberg, L. S., & Niparko, J. K. (2013). Effects of maternal sensitivity and cognitive and linguistic stimulation on cochlear implant users' language development over four years. The Journal of Pediatrics, 162, 343–348.
Rathmann, P. (1994). Good night, gorilla. New York, NY: G. P. Putnam's Sons.
Reynell, J., & Gruber, C. (1990). Reynell Developmental Language Scales. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (2002). Leiter International Performance Scale–Revised (Leiter-R). Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting.
Schachter, F. F. (1979). Everyday mother talk to toddlers: Early intervention. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Cramer, B. B. (1984). Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 175–190.
Storkel, H. L. (2002). Restructuring of similarity neighbourhoods in the developing mental lexicon. Journal of Child Language, 29, 251–274.
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1987). The phoneme as a perceptuomotor structure. In A. Allport, D. G. MacKay, W. Prinz, & E. Scheerer (Eds.), Language perception and production: Relationships between listening, speaking, reading, and writing (pp. 67–84). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Swanson, M. R., Donovan, K., Paterson, S., Wolff, J. J., Parish-Morris, J., Meera, S. S., & Piven J for the IBIS Network. (2019). Early language exposure supports later language skills in infants with and without autism. Autism Research.
Szagun, G., & Stumper, B. (2012). Age or experience? The influence of age at implantation and social and linguistic environment on language development in children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 1640–1654.
Updike, C., & Thornburg, J. D. (1992). Reading skills and auditory processing ability in children with chronic otitis media in early childhood. Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology, 101, 530–537.
Vihman, M. M. (1991). Ontogeny of phonetic gestures: Speech production. In I. G. Mattingly & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Modularity and the motor theory of speech perception (pp. 69–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vihman, M. M., & Velleman, S. L. (1989). Phonological reorganization: A case study. Language and Speech, 32, 149–170.
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.
Waterson, N. (1971). Child phonology: A prosodic view. Journal of Linguistics, 7, 179–211.
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 7, 49–63.
Wilcox, K., & Morris, S. (1999). Children's speech intelligibility measure. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Xu, S., Liu, P., Chen, Y., Chen, Y., Zhang, W., Zhao, H., … Guo, X. (2018). Foxp2 regulates anatomical features that may be relevant for vocal behaviors and bipedal locomotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115, 8799–8804.
Zimmerman, F. J., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Christakis, D. A., Gray, S., & Yapanel, U. (2009). Teaching by listening: The importance of adult-child conversations to language development. Pediatrics, 124, 342–349.
Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002). Preschool Language Scale–Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
Volume 63Number 122 January 2020
Pages: 234-258
PubMed: 31834998

History

  • Received: May 7, 2019
  • Revised: Aug 9, 2019
  • Accepted: Sep 24, 2019
  • Published online: Dec 13, 2019
  • Published in issue: Jan 22, 2020

Permissions

Request permissions for this article.

Authors

Affiliations

Susan Nittrouer
Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, University of Florida, Gainesville
Joanna H. Lowenstein
Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, University of Florida, Gainesville
Joseph Antonelli
Department of Statistics, University of Florida, Gainesville

Notes

Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time of publication.
Correspondence to Susan Nittrouer: [email protected]
Editor-in-Chief: Sean M. Redmond
Editor: Emily Lund

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Article Metrics
View all metrics



Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Citing Literature

  • Supporting Children’s Communication Development through Parental Education, Parenting Styles and Practices [Working Title], 10.5772/intechopen.1006856, (2024).
  • How Do Enriched Speech Acoustics Support Language Acquisition in Children With Hearing Loss? A Narrative Review, Ear & Hearing, 10.1097/AUD.0000000000001606, (2024).
  • Family-Centered Early Intervention Deaf/Hard of Hearing (FCEI-DHH): Support Principles, Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 10.1093/deafed/enad039, 29, SI, (SI64-SI85), (2024).
  • Integrated Language Intervention for Children with Hearing Loss, Pediatric Cochlear Implantation, 10.1007/978-3-031-67188-3_24, (411-424), (2024).
  • Family Dynamics Impact Cochlear Implant Outcomes, Pediatric Cochlear Implantation, 10.1007/978-3-031-67188-3_18, (317-331), (2024).
  • Literacy in Early Childhood with Hearing Loss: Challenges and Strategies in Education, Journal of Childhood Development, 10.25217/jcd.v3i2.3840, 3, 2, (69-80), (2023).
  • O impacto da deficiência auditiva nas relações familiares de escolares usuários de dispositivos eletrônicos de amplificação sonora, Audiology - Communication Research, 10.1590/2317-6431-2023-2817pt, 28, (2023).
  • The impact of hearing impairment on family relationships of schoolchildren who use individual sound amplification devices, Audiology - Communication Research, 10.1590/2317-6431-2023-2817en, 28, (2023).
  • Quantity and Quality of Parental Linguistic Input to Young Children with Cochlear Implants: A Longitudinal Study, Communication Sciences & Disorders, 10.12963/csd.23981, 28, 3, (669-688), (2023).
  • The Influence of Caregiver Language on the Association Between Child Temperament and Spoken Language in Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing, Ear & Hearing, 10.1097/AUD.0000000000001378, 44, 6, (1367-1378), (2023).
  • See more

View Options

Sign In Options

ASHA member? If so, log in with your ASHA website credentials for full access.

Member Login

View options

PDF

View PDF

Full Text

View Full Text

Figures

Tables

Media

Share

Share

Copy the content Link

Share