Abstract
Purpose
In healthy speakers, the more frequent and probable a word is in its context, the shorter the word tends to be. This study investigated whether these probabilistic effects were similarly sized for speakers with dysarthria of different severities.
Method
Fifty-six speakers of New Zealand English (42 speakers with dysarthria and 14 healthy speakers) were recorded reading the Grandfather Passage. Measurements of word duration, frequency, and transitional word probability were taken.
Results
As hypothesized, words with a higher frequency and probability tended to be shorter in duration. There was also a significant interaction between word frequency and speech severity. This indicated that the more severe the dysarthria, the smaller the effects of word frequency on speakers' word durations. Transitional word probability also interacted with speech severity, but did not account for significant unique variance in the full model.
Conclusions
These results suggest that, as the severity of dysarthria increases, the duration of words is less affected by probabilistic variables. These findings may be due to reductions in the control and execution of muscle movement exhibited by speakers with dysarthria.

References
-
Anderson, A. H., & Howarth, B. (2002). Referential form and word duration in video-mediated and face-to-face dialogues.InA. Pease, S. Colton, A. Smaill, & J. Lee (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th Workshop on the Semantics and Pragmatics of Dialogue. pp. (13–20). EDILOG. -
Aylett, M., & Turk, A. (2004). The smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: A functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence, and duration in spontaneous speech.Language and Speech, 47(1), 31–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309040470010201 -
Aylett, M., & Turk, A. (2006). Language redundancy predicts syllabic duration and the spectral characteristics of vocalic syllable nuclei.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(5), 3048–3058. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2188331 -
Baayen, R., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). CELEX2 LDC96L14. Linguistic Data Consortium. -
Baayen, R. H., & Shafaei-Bajestan, E. (2019). languageR: Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics. R package Version 1.5.0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=languageR -
Baker, R. E., & Bradlow, A. R. (2009). Variability in word duration as a function of probability, speech style, and prosody.Language and Speech, 52(4), 391–413. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830909336575 -
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4.Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 -
Bell, A., Brenier, J. M., Gregory, M., Girand, C., & Jurafsky, D. (2009). Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English.Journal of Memory and Language, 60(1), 92–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.003 -
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2012). Praat.Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.3.04)[Computer software] . https://www.praat.org -
Bradlow, A. R. (2002). Confluent talker-and listener-oriented forces in clear speech production.Laboratory Phonology, 7. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110197105.241 -
Brants, T., & Franz, A. (2006). Web 1T 5-gram corpus version 1.1[Computer software] .. Google Inc. https://catalog.ldc.upenn.edu/LDC2006T13 -
Chiu, Y. F., & Forrest, K. (2017). The interaction of lexical characteristics and speech production in Parkinson's disease.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(1), 13–23. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-S-15-0333 -
Connaghan, K. P., & Patel, R. (2017). The impact of contrastive stress on vowel acoustics and intelligibility in dysarthria.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(1), 38–50. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-S-15-0291 -
Duffy, J. R. (2013). Motor speech disorders: Substrates, differential diagnosis, and management (3rd ed.). Elsevier Mosby. -
Fletcher, A. R., McAuliffe, M. J., Lansford, K. L., & Liss, J. M. (2017). Assessing vowel centralization in dysarthria: A comparison of methods.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(2), 341–354. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-S-15-0355 -
Fletcher, A. R., McAuliffe, M. J., Lansford, K. L., Sinex, D. G., & Liss, J. M. (2017). Predicting intelligibility gains in individuals with dysarthria from baseline speech features.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(11), 3043–3057. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-S-16-0218 -
Fletcher, A. R., Wisler, A. A., McAuliffe, M. J., Lansford, K. L., & Liss, J. M. (2017). Predicting intelligibility gains in dysarthria through automated speech feature analysis.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(11), 3058–3068. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-16-0453 -
Fox, J. (2003). Effect displays in R for generalised linear models.Journal of Statistical Software, 8(15), 1–27. http://www.jstatsoft.org/v08/i15/ -
Fromont, R., & Hay, J. (2008). ONZE Miner: The development of a browser-based research tool.Corpora, 3(2), 173–193. https://doi.org/10.3366/E1749503208000142 -
Gahl, S., Yao, Y., & Johnson, K. (2012). Why reduce? Phonological neighborhood density and phonetic reduction in spontaneous speech.Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 789–806. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.11.006 -
Goldrick, M., McClain, R., Cibelli, E., Adi, Y., Gustafson, E., Moers, C., & Keshet, J. (2019). The influence of lexical selection disruptions on articulation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(6), 1107–1141. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000633 -
Hartelius, L., Runmarker, B., Andersen, O., & Nord, L. (2000). Temporal speech characteristics of individuals with multiple sclerosis and ataxic dysarthria: ‘Scanning speech’ revisited.Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 52(5), 228–238. https://doi.org/10.1159/000021538 -
Hawker, T., Gardiner, M., & Bennetts, A. (2007). Practical queries of a massive n-gram database.InN. Colineau & M. Dras (Eds.), Proceedings of the Australasian Language Technology Workshop 2007. (pp. 40–48). ACL Anthology. https://www.aclweb.org/anthology/U07-1008 -
Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, W. J. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 824–843. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.824 -
Johnson, K. (2004).Massive reduction in conversational American English. In spontaneous speech: Data and analysis . InK. Yoneyama & K. Maekawa (Eds.), Proceedings of the 1st session of the 10th International Symposium (pp. 29–54). The National International Institute for Japanese Language. -
Jurafsky, D., Bell, A., Gregory, M., & Raymond, W. D. (2001). Probabilistic relations between words: Evidence from reduction in lexical production.InJ. L. Bybee & P. J. Hopper (Eds.) (Vol. frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure pp. 229–254). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.45.13jur -
Kim, H., Hasegawa-Johnson, M., & Perlman, A. (2010, May). Acoustic cues to lexical stress in spastic dysarthria. Paper presented at Speech Prosody 2010, Fifth International Conference, Chicago, IL, USa. -
Kim, Y., Kent, R. D., & Weismer, G. (2011). An acoustic study of the relationships among neurologic disease, dysarthria type, and severity of dysarthria.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(2), 417–429. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0020) -
Klatt, D. H. (1976). Linguistic uses of segmental duration in English: Acoustic and perceptual evidence.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 59(5), 1208–1221. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.380986 -
Lieberman, P. (1963). Some effects of semantic and grammatical context on the production and perception of speech.Language and Speech, 6(3), 172–187. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383096300600306 -
Liss, J. M., Spitzer, S., Caviness, J. N., Adler, C., & Edwards, B. (1998). Syllabic strength and lexical boundary decisions in the perception of hypokinetic dysarthric speech.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 104(4), 2457–2466. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.423753 -
Liss, J. M., & Weismer, G. (1994). Selected acoustic characteristics of contrastive stress production in control geriatric, apraxic, and ataxic dysarthric speakers.Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 8(1), 45–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699209408985574 -
Liss, J. M., White, L., Mattys, S. L., Lansford, K., Lotto, A. J., Spitzer, S. M., & Caviness, J. N. (2009). Quantifying speech rhythm abnormalities in the dysarthrias.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(5), 1334–1352. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0208) -
Mattys, S. L., White, L., & Melhorn, J. F. (2005). Integration of multiple speech segmentation cues: A hierarchical framework.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(4), 477–500. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.477 -
McHenry, M. (1998). The ability to effect intended stress following traumatic brain injury.Brain Injury, 12(6), 495–503. https://doi.org/10.1080/026990598122458 -
Moers, C., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. (2015, August). Probabilistic reduction in reading aloud: A comparison of younger and older adults.Paper presented at the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS 2015), Glasgow, United Kingdom . -
Moers, C., Meyer, A., & Janse, E. (2016). Effects of word frequency and transitional probability on word reading durations of younger and older speakers.Language and Speech, 60(2), 289–317. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830916649215 -
Myers, B. R., & Watson, D. G. (2019). Paying the meter: Effect of metrical similarity on word lengthening.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(6), 1941–1947. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01635-4 - R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
-
Robb, M. P., Maclagan, M. A., & Chen, Y. (2004). Speaking rates of American and New Zealand varieties of English.Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 18(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/0269920031000105336 -
Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, C., & Scarborough, H. S. (1977). Frequency and repetition effects in lexical memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.3.1.1 -
Sheard, C., Adams, R. D., & Davis, P. J. (1991). Reliability and agreement of ratings of ataxic dysarthric speech samples with varying intelligibility.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34(2), 285–293. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3402.85 -
Smiljanić, R., & Bradlow, A. R. (2009). Speaking and hearing clearly: Talker and listener factors in speaking style changes.Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 236–264. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00112.x -
Spitzer, S. M., Liss, J. M., & Mattys, S. L. (2007). Acoustic cues to lexical segmentation: A study of resynthesized speech.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 122(6), 3678–3687. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2801545 -
Vitevitch, M. S., Armbrüster, J., & Chu, S. (2004). Sublexical and lexical representations in speech production: Effects of phonotactic probability and onset density.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(2), 514–529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.514 -
Watson, P. J., & Munson, B. (2008). Parkinson's disease and the effect of lexical factors on vowel articulation.The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(5), EL291–EL295. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2987464 -
Young, S., Evermann, G., Kershaw, D., Moore, G., Odell, J., Ollason, D., Povey, D., Valtchev, V., & Woodland, P. (2002). The HTK-Book 3.2. Cambridge University. -
Ziegler, W., Hartmann, E., & Hoole, P. (1993). Syllabic timing in dysarthria.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(4), 683–693. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3604.683