Active word learning in children mostly seems an easy or even “automatic” process, although typically developing children show great variability in the speed and number of words they acquire during the preschool period (
Klee & Harrison, 2001). Around the age of 2 years, the average amount of vocabulary knowledge corresponds to 200 words. Around the age of 3 years, the estimated average is 1,000 words, and at the end of the preschool period (5 years old), it is 3,000 words (
Schaerlaekens, 2008). During the grades from kindergarten to Grade 2, the difference between children with small and large vocabularies continues to get larger, and this gap is seldomly eliminated by education alone (
Stone, 2004).
The acquisition of receptive and expressive vocabulary, both in children and in adults, does not stand alone but shows interaction with other cognitive processes such as auditory verbal short-term memory (STM;
Buchsbaum, 2016) but also long-term memory, controlled attention, and fluid intelligence (
Gillam et al., 2021). Different assessments have been developed to specifically examine receptive and expressive vocabulary in young Dutch children, but these tests measure generally vocabulary size, rather than the learning process (
Schlichting & Spelberg, 2012;
Schlichting, 2004;
Semel et al., 2012). Implementing verbal memory tests (both with known words and nonwords) in the assessment of language and developmental disorders can also provide a wealth of information on how familiar and novel verbal material is processed. By assessing this memory capacity before children start in elementary school, strengths and weaknesses in children's skills can be observed and intervention programs can be installed where needed. In appraising the literature on verbal memory assessment in young children, however, we found that assessment of word list learning, for the Dutch population, starts at the age of 6 or 7 years (
Kemp, 2011;
Van den Burg & Kingma, 1999;
Kingma, 2021), with no norms available for younger children. This study addresses this gap. We developed and validated a novel auditory verbal learning instrument for 4- and 5-year-old children that may help clinicians in diagnosing and treating children with language impairment and/or deficits in verbal learning and memory.
From Memory to Word Learning in Typically Developing Children
Human memory can be described as an information-processing system where new memories are created based on three main mechanisms: encoding, storage, and retrieval. In modern cognitive psychology,
STM is defined as “a cognitive faculty that enables humans and other complex animals to temporarily store, process and manipulate important pieces of information that are no longer readily available in the sensory environment” (
Buchsbaum, 2016, p. 863). Whereas
phonological STM refers to the simple storage of information in phonological form, it can be partially equated to the phonological loop described within Baddeley's working memory system (
Baddeley, 2000), comprising four important elements. The phonological loop is responsible for storage and maintenance of information in a verbal form. This phonological loop consists of a phonological short-term store and a subvocal rehearsal process. Before 7 years of age, however, spontaneous rehearsal does not reliably occur. In younger children, the phonological loop therefore consists of the phonological store only (
Gathercole et al., 2004). The phonological loop was initially termed the
articulatory loop by
Baddeley and Hitch (2019); later, they abandoned the term since they regarded articulation as the method of rehearsal rather than the storage medium itself. The visuospatial sketchpad is dedicated to the storage and maintenance of visuospatial information. The episodic buffer combines all verbal and visuospatial information into complex multimodal representations linking working memory to long-term memory (
Repovs & Baddeley, 2006). The “central executive” is seen as a control system of limited attentional capacity that coordinates and controls these three slave systems.
Memories encoded can be translated onto the long-term memory system. This system has two components: Explicit/declarative memory refers to memories involving personal experiences (episodic memory) and factual information that we can consciously retrieve and intentionally articulate (semantic memory); implicit memory refers to perceptive and emotional unconscious memories that influence our behavior (
Dew & Cabeza, 2011). Memory retrieval is the process of remembering information stored in long-term memory.
How does this memory system support word learning? Words are the core elements of language, represented in at least three components: meaning (or concept), lemma (grammatical characteristics), and word form (how the word sounds; e.g.,
Levelt et al., 1999). In order to learn a new word and assign a meaning to it, children have to complete at least the following three tasks: (a) perceive and segment a string of sounds (phonological form) from the stream of speech they hear, (b) hold this phonological form in STM while the lexicon is searched, and (c) identify a meaning and pair that to the new word form (
Brackenbury & Pye, 2005). According to
Martin and Gupta (2004) and
Gupta and Tisdale (2009), the learning of phonological word forms depends on phonological STM and on the contribution of semantic memory. These links will be further detailed in the following paragraphs.
The first process named by
Brackenbury and Pye (2005), perceiving and segmenting a string of sounds, relies mainly on processes of auditory perception and recognition of acoustic and prosodic patterns. These are critical to generate the input to Step 2 mentioned by the same authors. Here, the contribution of phonological STM to learning a new word is critical. As a string of sounds is heard, there is an automatic lexical search (
Dell, 1997) during which the phonological information has to remain actively available in STM (
Gupta & Tisdale, 2009;
Martin & Gupta, 2004).
Importantly, forgetting of all or a part of the information kept in phonological STM during word learning means that the new lexical form cannot be stored correctly in the lexicon. In line with this, studies on the relation between vocabulary development and STM in preschool children between 4 and 6 years old (
Majerus et al., 2006;
Messer et al., 2010) have shown an important bidirectional influence between STM capacity and vocabulary development. A strong STM enables the child to form stable and precise long-term phonological representations. Conversely, as a child's vocabulary knowledge increases, the phonological representations of words become more fine-grained (more segmental rather than holistic in form), a process known as lexical restructuring (
Metsala, 1999). This enhanced phonological awareness allows a more accurate encoding of new words in STM.
Continuing to Step 3 described by
Brackenbury and Pie (2005), existing strings of sounds are identified as existing lexical (vocabulary) units and can be linked to their respective meanings, both of which are stored within semantic memory (
Gupta & Tisdale, 2009;
Martin & Gupta, 2004). However, strings of sounds that cannot be found in the phonological lexicon (novel words or nonwords) trigger the search for a meaning.
Carey and Bartlett (1978) originally stated that the mapping process required to assign a heard word form to a meaning occurs in two phases: (a) the initial mapping of a linguistic label to a real-world entity (fast mapping) and (b) the subsequent retention and development of the initial representation (extended mapping;
Vlach & Sandhofer, 2012). Fast mapping thus requires some kind of world knowledge (i.e., semantic memory), which will be linked to a new label. However, extended mapping enriches the semantic representation of the concept with critical distinctions between ordinate and subordinate entities, thus determining its correct category extension (
Carey, 2010). Critically, in order to use either of these processes to learn words successfully, access to and retention in semantic memory are needed.
Given that novel words with their respective phonological and semantic information are stored in vocabulary in an interactive network (
McClelland et al., 1995), the number of nouns a children already have in their vocabulary affects their ability in comprehension and expression tasks. The larger and more robust the network, the more easily new memories are encoded and retained.
Walker and McGregor (2013) emphasize that the vocabulary size has a causal effect on learning new words: A good word learner builds a large vocabulary, which, in turn, enables additional learning. The concept of “lexical restructuring” (
Metsala, 1999) provides a clear rationale for how the developmental refining of phonological knowledge may support learning. When children first encounter new words, they are represented in a holistic manner at the lexical level. As their vocabulary grows, children develop phonotactic knowledge, which makes them more sensitive to the phonologically finer grained structures within words, and they adopt the phoneme as the basic unit of oral language processing. Even in preschool children, who have no literacy experience, phonological representations become more accurate and more segmented as they develop (
Ainsworth et al., 2016), leading to more durable representations in the phonological loop.
Assessment of Vocabulary Knowledge and Verbal Memory
Assessment of vocabulary knowledge in a clinical or educational setting is typically done through static assessments, where children name images (
Semel et al., 2012) or point to images that correspond to verbally presented words (e.g.,
Schlichting, 2004). While these tools are important to measure the vocabulary size and can give a good indication of a child's capacity compared to peers, they give little information about the child's potential learning ability or about which aspects of learning may be difficult. Furthermore, such assessments are influenced by linguistic and cultural biases, being less suitable for socially diverse settings, second-language learners, and bilingual individuals (
Hasson et al., 2013). A different approach is dynamic assessment. Here, a child is given the opportunity to learn a certain behavior during a learning phase of the assessment. Subsequently, and usually within the same session, the product of that learning is tested. Because the learning process, and not prior knowledge, is the assessed function, this type of examination is less affected by cultural or linguistic biases.
Verbal learning is, by definition, a dynamic concept and thus is examined by using dynamic assessment. To understand the nature of novel word learning, different approaches exist.
Yu and Ballard (2007), for example, developed a unified model of early word learning, more specifically a computational model that is able to discover spoken words from continuous speech and associate them with their perceptually grounded meanings. In the field of linguistics and language development, verbal learning is often studied using tasks where a novel word (e.g.,
pilking) is presented within a specific experimental manipulation (e.g.,
Arunachalam & Waxman, 2011). Often, the focus is on examining what information children use to derive initial (fast mapped) word meanings (e.g., syntactic and semantic contexts) or what variables affect the success in learning a new word (e.g., number and frequency of exposure;
Carey, 2010). Only recently did these types of tasks transfer to clinical practice within the Quick Interactive Language Screener (
Levine et al., 2020).
The latest version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fifth Edition (WISC-V;
Wechsler, 2014) provides a symbol translation test aimed to detect difficulties in visual–verbal associative memory (visual–verbal pairs have to be learned), measured in an immediate, delayed, and recognition trial. This task differs from a word list learning task. As described in the WISC-V Technical and Interpretive Manual (
Wechsler, 2014), visual–verbal associative memory tasks similar to the symbol translation subtests are closely associated with reading decoding skills, word reading accuracy and fluency, text reading, and reading comprehension. They can support the nature of diagnosing learning disabilities but are less appropriate in defining the nature of developmental language disorders.
Another strategy to examine verbal learning is via an auditory verbal learning test (AVLT), an approach initially developed by Rey (
1941,
1958) and adapted widely in different forms and languages (
Vakil et al., 2012;
Van den Burg & Kingma, 1999, 2021). In the Dutch adaptation of the AVLT for children aged 6–13 years, children are presented with 15 existing nouns, over five learning trials (
Van den Burg & Kingma, 1999, 2021). Each time, the child is asked to recall as many words as possible. Following a 25-min interval, the child is again asked to complete a delayed recall trial and, after that, a recognition trial. During the recognition trial, the words learned are presented along with distractors, and the child indicates whether or not those words were part of the learning set. This task is well known and widely used in the Netherlands both in clinical practice and in research.
Novel word learning and verbal list learning share several processes; both tasks require the perception of a phonological form and the storage of that form in the phonological STM. Furthermore, both tasks prompt a search in the lexical-semantic system. In the context of verbal list learning, the lexical search leads to reactivation of the existing lexeme, which corresponds to the concept, making it easier to activate (
Paradis, 2009). In the case of novel word learning, a lexical search does not identify existing lexemes directly, although when presented along with existing concepts, a semantic representation can be retrieved. The new lexeme can then be mapped onto the existing concept (
Green, 1998).
Implementing an auditory-verbal memory test in the diagnostic process of young children with language problems can lead to a better understanding of encoding, storage, and retrieval abilities necessary for word learning (
Archibald, 2017;
Jackson et al., 2021). The administration of an AVLT provides a wealth of information (
Lezak, 1995): immediate word span under overload conditions (Trial 1), final acquisition level (Trial 5), total acquisition and learning curve (Trials 1–5), delayed recall after 20–30 min, recognition out of a list with distractor items, number of repetitions, and number and types of intrusions.
Several word list tasks have been designed and validated for younger children, including normative data for immediate and delayed recall and recognition. The Californian Verbal Learning Test for Children (CVLT-C;
Delis et al., 1994) is an examination of auditory and verbal learning for children between the ages of 5 years and 16 years 11 months.
Goodman et al. (1999) presented normative data for 4-year-old children on the CVLT-C. The Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning (
Sheslow & Adams, 1990) also includes a verbal list learning task starting at the age of 5 years. The Children's Memory Scale (
Cohen, 1997) assesses auditory-verbal learning from ages 5 through 16 years. The three forementioned list learning tests all make use of 15 words or more in their list learning task (
Kreutzer et al., 2011). They were developed for a large age span, which makes the test potentially less suitable for the youngest age groups. By reducing the number of words for the 10-word test for kids (10WT-K), this task is better suited to young children who have a lower attention span, among other yet immature cognitive functions, compared to older subjects. We also choose not to administer an interference list, given the limited executive functioning abilities in young children.
In this study, in comparison to the standard AVLT for children and adults, a new recognition format is added. In neuropsychological assessment, the recognition score can be used to distinguish between retrieval and storage limitations. Children who encounter difficulties in storage generally show an insufficient score on both delayed recall and recognition, whereas children who exhibit retrieval difficulties show a better score on recognition than on delayed recall.
Kok and Kingma (2009) administered the recognition trial as part of the Dutch AVLT for children aged 6–13 years in 90 participants. The recognition lists included 15 distractor words with low association apart from the 15 stimulus words of the AVLT. An important finding of this study was the ceiling effect: 89% of the children obtained a high to maximal score (≥ 28/30). The 11% of the children who obtained low scores (≤ 27/30) also had low delayed recall scores, indicating storage problems. The ceiling effect for the recognition trial raised the question if this score is a valid measure to detect retrieval difficulties. These findings prompt the need to develop a recognition test with higher discrimination value.
Lezak (1995) emphasizes that use of words (and not digits for example) in AVLTs introduces the possibility to study effects of word properties on verbal learning. Following this suggestion, we added a second recognition trial to the 10WT-K with a high degree of semantic association between words that may provide a more valid approach than the current test.
An additional approach to assessment of storage and manipulation of phonological information is using unknown words, also known as nonwords. A task such as nonword repetition has shown high diagnostic accuracy to discriminate both monolinguals and bilinguals with developmental language disorder from typically developing children (
Boerma et al., 2015). Nonword repetition ability is the ability to repeat possible but nonoccurring word forms and, just like list learning, is a good measure of phonological STM capacity (
Coady & Evans, 2008), hence of an important aspect of the ability to learn new words. Furthermore, because nonwords are designed in such way that the phonological structures included in stimuli abide to the phonotactic characteristics of the language (e.g.,
Boerma et al., 2015), performance can be facilitated by this knowledge, just like in list learning tasks. Importantly, phonotactic knowledge is an important predictor of vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual children (
Messer et al., 2010).
For nonwords and existing words, the knowledge about the phonological structure of the native language appears to influence STM performance (
Majerus et al., 2006). However, nonword repetition tests also differ from word list tests. First, in nonword repetition tests, only the recall immediately after each individual item is examined, and children are exposed to the nonwords only once. Furthermore, lexical characteristics of the word other than phonotactic structure (e.g., lexical frequency and age of acquisition) and semantic properties (imageability and semantic category) do not play a role in performance on nonword repetition tests. This way, their potential contribution to verbal learning cannot be measured. In the CVLT-C (
Delis et al., 1994), for example, several words of one semantic category are included in order to measure clustering strategies. In our own proposed 10-word test, distractors in the recognition test include semantically related words in order to test recognition in a more complex setting.
Discussion
Using an auditory verbal learning task specifically developed for 4- and 5-year-old children, significant correlations were found between auditory verbal learning and three language measures: receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary, and nonword repetition. Different from already existing verbal learning tests such as the CVLT-C (
Delis et al., 1994), all 10 words in our parallel tasks came from a different semantic category, diminishing the opportunity to use semantic clustering strategies. This makes it possible to examine the child's learning ability in this auditory-verbal learning task with less possibility for the development of strategies that rely on other aspects of cognition. Furthermore, implementing a recognition trial with semantically related distractors can facilitate the detection of individual differences in this group of children. In the following paragraphs, we will further discuss the findings of our research, limitations, and future directions.
Relation Between Auditory-Verbal Learning/Memory and Vocabulary Achievement
The results of the first research question reveal significant positive correlations between receptive and expressive vocabulary knowledge and the total acquisition score on the verbal learning task, which is a measure of initial learning and consolidation. In the case of verbal list learning, we mainly measure the capacity of phonological STM. However, given the supraspan nature of the 10WT-K, attentional control provided by the working memory central executive is also activated (
McCabe et al., 2010). We found no correlations between vocabulary knowledge and the score on the delayed recall task.
The relationship we found between the auditory verbal learning process and vocabulary knowledge is known to be bidirectional. The better a new word can be rehearsed in phonological STM, the higher the probability that it will be transformed into a stable and precise long-term phonological representation (
Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989). On the other hand, vocabulary growth itself is also a causal factor responsible for the developmental increase in verbal STM capacity (
Martin, 2009;
Metsala, 1999).
In young developing children, results of this study also show a positive correlation between nonword repetition (PFB score) and the total acquisition score of the 10WT-K reflecting the importance of phonological STM as a language learning device. Especially in young children, this phonological component is of great importance in developing their vocabulary (
Baddeley, 2010). As the lexical representation of words in young children develop from a more holistic form to a more segmented form (lexical restructuring), phonological STM gains accuracy. Second, as children grow older, auditory verbal information becomes more semantically encoded because of the growth of their semantic network; they can associate new verbal material to already existing knowledge. Therefore, older children will be able to retain more auditory verbal material in their STM (
Njiokiktjien, 2020). This growth in STM capacity is reflected in our descriptive data of the 10WT-K average total acquisition score, which shows an increase between 4- and 5-year-olds (from 20.92 to 29.29). The findings of correlations between the 10WT-K and the three aforementioned language measures support the construct validity of this new test to evaluate word learning mechanisms important for vocabulary development.
The positive correlation found between the ability of a child to repeat nonwords and the total score on the 10WT-K recall emphasizes that phonological STM capacity is important to support the encoding process in order to recall information correctly after a short period of time. Children who obtained a high score on the recognition task also performed better at repeating nonwords.
The Role of Error Types
The second research question focused on the occurrence of errors, more specifically intrusions and repetitions. No correlations were found between these error types and scores on expressive or receptive vocabulary. There is thus no evidence that these measures reflect processes important for language development, as previously shown by
Van den Burg and Kingma (1999). Rather, these parameters are often interesting to study executive functioning (
Kittler et al., 2006;
Lezak, 1995). If it is indeed the case that intrusions and repetitions occur more frequently in children with poor executive abilities, the normative data of the 10WT-K can be helpful in the differential diagnosis of this clinical group.
An important remark to make here is that the occurrence of repetitions might be, to a certain extent, generated by the instructions given, as children heard the following message: “You must also say the words you have mentioned before.” Although this statement refers to words recalled in previous trials, this time reference may be unclear to some children. This is because the abstract sense of time starts to develop as from the age of 4–5 years and continues to develop until the age of 11 years (
Njiokiktjien, 2020). Hence, some children may have interpreted this to mean that they could say words mentioned earlier during the same trial.
Recognition Trial: Toward a More Valid Approach
When we encode known verbal information (such as the 10 words in our word list), we predominantly encode the meaning/concept of the word (
Cremona et al., 2020;
Jefferies et al., 2006;
Meltzer et al., 2016). This concept is defined by its relationship to other concepts in the semantic network, which give the concept its meaning (
Vingerhoets & Lannoo, 1998). Difficulties between encoding and retrieval of these concepts can be observed by means of the recognition trial of an AVLT.
In the current Dutch AVLT for 6–13 years of age (
Van den Burg & Kingma, 1999, 2021), children often perform at ceiling level on the recognition trial, making it difficult to discern between encoding and retrieval difficulties. Therefore, in this study, a different approach was explored by comparing two types of recognition tests: one list with items that are semantically related to the target words and one list with items that have no semantic relationship with the target words. Comparing the results of both lists revealed significant differences as well in total score (sum of correct answers) as in false-positive and false-negative ratio. Children who received a recognition task with semantically related items obtained a lower number of correct answers and showed more frequent false-positive and false-negative answers than children who received a recognition task with semantically unrelated items. As observed in the abovementioned Dutch AVLT for 6–13 years of age (
Van den Burg & Kingma, 1999, 2021), most scores in the version with semantically unrelated items reached ceiling level. These results indicate that when a word has to be selected among semantically related distractors, there is greater activation of shared semantic features, posing greater demands.
By introducing a recognition task that is more demanding for children, a greater variability was seen in test results, making it possible to detect individual differences. Future studies should address the question if validity is higher with this approach. Also, it can be interesting to observe if these differences in performance between both recognition trials also exist in older children, between 6 and 12 years old.
When reviewing similar recognition tasks used for memory assessment in children such as the CVLT-C (
Delis et al., 1994;
Fahrer & Drozdick, 2020;
Goodman et al., 1999), no ceiling effects in recognition tasks are reported in younger age groups. A possible explanation is that the recognition trial of the CVLT-C is more demanding due to the use of an interference list (List B). Hence, the total amount of words in their recognition list is longer compared to our recognition task. Furthermore, by calculating a “total recognition discriminability score,” they combine both hits and false-positive scores, leading to a more accurate measure to examine whether the examinee could distinguish between target and nontarget words from List A (
Farrer & Drozdick, 2020).
Limitations
Limitations to our study can be found in assessing the youngest group of children (4;0–4;6 [years;months]). As we administered the 10WT-K by means of an audio recording, some of these children experienced difficulties in auditory discrimination (e.g., park/ark/hark). Observing a speaker's articulatory patterns substantially improves the intelligibility of spoken speech; multisensory integration of speech inputs is important to effective communication (
Ross et al., 2011). Although the use of an audio recording is necessary in terms of standardization, the option of reading the words out loud can be considered as this is a more natural way to present the words.
Second, a low sustained and/or focused attention sometimes interfered with performance throughout the group. Controlling for this factor in our group of participants could be an option, although attention is difficult to measure in this young age and would have to rely on questionnaires.
Furthermore, a remark can be made regarding the use of the receptive vocabulary items of the WPPSI-III-NL. We noticed that certain items are outdated, leading to errors that may be acceptable. For example, when a child has to select the target word “to pay,” several children pointed at a person facing the computer, which is also a possible way to pay. We have removed this item in our test sample. As an alternative, we could choose a receptive and expressive vocabulary task that belongs to the same assessment battery, for example, the Schlichting Test for Language Reception and Production (
Schlichting & Spelberg, 2012).
We note that the use of Bonferroni corrections may increase the possibility of beta error. Therefore, we advise caution in interpreting nonsignificant results.
Implications and Future Directions
First, this study contributes to the pediatric neuropsychological practice by developing a feasible auditory verbal learning task for young children. The vast majority of the participants understood the purpose of the task and cooperated well. Reliable results could be obtained in almost all children. Only one participant was excluded from the study due to a recently detected hearing disorder. Furthermore, this test could be used in older children who cannot pass the standard Dutch AVLT for children. Particularly in 6-year-old children, a list including 15 words occasionally seems too long. Although no normative data were acquired in 6 years olds part of the 10WT-K data can be used in a relative way in slightly older children, such as slope of the learning curve, difference between the score on learning Trial 5 and delayed recall, and the difference between the total score and the recognition score. Such relative use of normative data can also be considered in children with attention disorders or lower verbal intelligence.
This tool can be helpful in evaluating children with different neurological diseases, developmental disorders, or learning disabilities who often show deficits in auditory-verbal learning (
Archibald & Gathercole, 2006;
Kingma et al., 1993;
Nagel et al., 2006;
Riccio et al., 2007). By characterizing difficulties in phonological STM in children with language difficulties at a young age, more efficient manners of instruction can be implemented in educating these children.
Not only in clinical populations, but also in typically developing children, the 10WT-K can contribute to understanding the relation between language and memory functions. Longitudinal monitoring of the children in this study could shed light on the predictive value of the present 10WT-K regarding school results that are related to language skills at a later age (e.g., reading comprehension, critical listening, or spelling). Over the past years, early childhood education is increasingly faced with children speaking multiple languages, resulting in new challenges to ensure their inclusion and learning opportunities in the classroom (
Langeloo et al., 2021). Future research could explore if this 10WT-K can be implemented in the longitudinal, dynamic assessment of multilingual children (
Hasson et al., 2013).
Construct validity was established by correlating the total acquisition score with relevant language measures such as receptive and expressive vocabulary. Furthermore, by developing a recognition trial that is more demanding for children, differentiation between encoding and retrieval deficits will be easier to detect compared to when using existing instruments. Of course, in order to use this test in clinical settings, further research on validity and reliability is necessary.
Author Contributions
Ann Kasperek: Conceptualization (Equal), Methodology (Equal), Formal analysis (Equal), Investigation (Lead), Data curation (Equal), Writing – Original draft (Lead), Visualization (Equal), Project Administration (Equal). Vânia de Aguiar: Conceptualization (Equal), Methodology (Equal), Formal analysis (Equal), Resources (Equal), Data curation (Equal), Writing – Review & Editing (Equal), Visualization (Equal), Supervision (Lead), Project Administration (Equal), Funding acquisition (Lead). Annet Kingma: Conceptualization (Equal), Methodology (Equal), Resources (Equal), Writing – Review & Editing (Equal), Visualization (Supporting), Supervision (Supporting).